Personality Article Reviews

Ace your studies with our custom writing services! We've got your back for top grades and timely submissions, so you can say goodbye to the stress. Trust us to get you there!


Order a Similar Paper Order a Different Paper

 

A good article review will include the following:

  • A ½ page (minimum) review of the main ideas the article discusses and
  • A ½ page (minimum) personal reflection on the article consisting of your personal opinions, thoughts, and/or experiences related to the topic

Your complete assignment will be 1 page per article, at least 2 pages total.

PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
1991,44

THE BIG FIVE PERSONALITY DIMENSIONS AND JOB
PERFORMANCE: A META-ANALYSIS

MURRAY R. BARRICK, MICHAEL K. MOUNT
Department of Management and Organizations

University of Iowa

This study investigated the relation of the “Big Five” personality di-
mensions (Extraversion, Emotional Stability, Agreeableness, Consci-
entiousness, and Openness to Experience) to three job performance
criteria (job proficiency, training proficiency, and personnel data) for
five occupational groups (professionals, police, managers, sales, and
skilled/semi-skilled). Results indicated that one dimension of person-
ality. Conscientiousness, showed consistent relations with all job per-
formance criteria for all occupational groups. For the remaining per-
sonality dimensions, the estimated true score correlations varied by
occupational group and criterion type. Extraversion was a valid pre-
dictor for two occupations involving social interaction, managers and
sales (across criterion types). Also, both Openness to Experience and
Extraversion were valid predictors of the training proficiency criterion
(across occupations). Other personality dimensions were also found
to be valid predictors for some occupations and some criterion types,
but the magnitude of the estimated true score correlations was small
(p < .10). Overall, the results illustrate the benefits of using the 5-
factor model of personality to accumulate and communicate empirical
findings. The findings have numerous implications for research and
practice in personnel psychology, especially in the subfields of person-
nel selection, training and development, and performance appraisal.

Introduction

Over the past 25 years, a number of researchers have investigated the
validity of personality measures for personnel selection purposes. The
overall conclusion from these studies is that the validity of personality as
a predictor of job performance is quite low (e.g., Ghiselli, 1973; Guion
& Gottier, 1965; Locke & Hulin, 1962; Reilly & Chao, 1982; Schmitt,

Both authors contributed equally to this study. We would like to thank Frank Schmidt,
Ralph Alexander, Paul Costa, Mike Judiesch, Wendy Dunn, and Jacob Sines for thoughtful
comments about the article and some of the data analyses. We gratefully acknowledge
the assistance of Mike Judiesch, Wendy Dunn, Eric Neumann, Val Arnold, and Duane
Thompson in categorizing the personality scales.

Correspondence and requests for reprints should be addressed to Murray R. Barrick,
Department of Management and Organizations, College of Business Administration, The
University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242.

COPYRIGHT © 1991 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY. INC

1

2 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY

Gooding, Noe, & Kirsch, 1984). However, at the time these studies were
conducted, no well-accepted taxonomy existed for classifying personality
traits. Consequently, it was not possible to determine whether there
were consistent, meaningful relationships between particular personality
constructs and performance criteria in different occupations.

In the past 10 years, the views of many personalify psychologists have
converged regarding the structure and concepts of personalify. Gener-
ally, researchers agree that there are five robust factors of personalify
(described below) which can serve as a meaningful taxonomy for classi-
fying personalify attributes (Digman, 1990). Our purpose in the present
study is to examine the relationship of these five personalify constructs
to job performance measures for different occupations, rather than to
focus on the overall validify of personalify as previous researchers have
done.

Emergence of the 5-Factor Model

Systematic efforts to organize the taxonomy of personalify began
shortly after McDougall (1932) wrote that, “Personalify may to advan-
tage be broadly analyzed into five distinguishable but separate factors,
namely intellect, character, temperament, disposition, and temper…”
(p. 15). About 10 years later, Cattell (1943, 1946, 1947, 1948) devel-
oped a relatively complex taxonomy of individual differences that con-
sisted of 16 primary factors and 8 second-order factors. However, re-
peated attempts by researchers to replicate his work were unsuccessful
(Fiske, 1949; Tupes, 1957; Tupes & Christal, 1961) and, in each case,
researchers found that the 5-factor model accounted for the data quite
well. For example, Tupes and Christal (1961) reanalyzed the correlations
reported by Cattell and Fiske and found that there was good support for
five factors: Surgency, Emotional Stabilify, Agreeableness, Dependabil-
ify, and Culture. As it would turn out later, these factors (and those of
McDougall 35 years before) were remarkably similar to those generally
accepted by researchers today. However, as Digman (1990) points out,
the work of Tupes and Christal had only a minor impact because their
study was published in an obscure Air Force technical report. The 5-
factor model obtained by Fiske (1949) and Tupes and Christal (1961)
was corroborated in four subsequent studies (Borgatta, 1964; Hakel,
1974; Norman, 1963; Smith 1967). Borgatta’s findings are noteworthy
because he obtained five stable factors across five methods of data gath-
ering. Norman’s work is especially significant because his labels (Ex-
traversion. Emotional Stabilify, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and
Culture) are used commonly in the literature and have been referred to,
subsequently, as “Norman’s Big Five” or simply as the “Big Five.”

BARRICK AND MOUNT 3

During the past decade, an impressive body of literature has accu-
mulated which provides compelling evidence for the robustness of the 5-
factor model: across different theoretical frameworks (Goldberg, 1981);
using different instruments (e.g., Conley, 1985; Costa & McCrae, 1988;
Lorr & Youniss, 1973; McCrae, 1989; McCrae & Costa, 1985, 1987,
1989); in different cultures (e.g.. Bond, Nakazato, & Shiraishi, 1975;
Noller, Law, & Comrey, 1987); using ratings obtained from different
sources (e.g., Digman & Inouye, 1986; Digman & Takemoto-Chock,
1981; Fiske, 1949; McCrae & Costa, 1987; Norman, 1963; Norman &
Goldberg, 1966; Watson, 1989); and with a variety of samples (see Dig-
man, 1990, for a more detailed discussion). An important consideration
for the field of personnel psychology is that these dimensions are also rel-
atively independent of measures of cognitive ability (McCrae & Costa,
1987).

It should be pointed out that some researchers have reservations
about the 5-factor model, particularly the imprecise specification of
these dimensions (Briggs, 1989; John, 1989; Livneh & Livneh, 1989;
Waller & Ben-Porath, 1987). Some researchers suggest that more than
five dimensions are needed to encompass the domain of personality. For
example, Hogan (1986) advocates six dimensions (Sociability, Ambition,
Adjustment, Likability, Prudence, and Intellectance). The principle dif-
ference seems to be the splitting of the Extraversion dimension into So-
ciability and Ambition.

Interpretations of the “Big Five”

While there is general agreement among researchers concerning the
number of factors, there is some disagreement about their precise mean-
ing, particularly Norman’s Conscientiousness and Culture factors. Of
course, some variation from study to study is to be expected with factors
as broad and inclusive as the 5-factor model. As shown below, however,
there is a great deal of commonality in the traits that define each factor,
even though the name attached to the factor differs.

It is widely agreed that the first dimension is Eysenck’s Extraver-
sion/Intraversion. Most frequently this dimension has been called Ex-
traversion or Surgency (Botwin & Buss, 1989; Digman & Takemoto-
Chock, 1981; Hakel, 1974; Hogan, 1983; Howarth, 1976; John, 1989;
Krug & Johns, 1986; McCrae & Costa, 1985; Noller et al., 1987; Nor-
man, 1963; Smith, 1967). Traits frequently associated with it include be-
ing sociable, gregarious, assertive, talkative, and active. As mentioned
above, Hogan (1986) interprets this dimension as consisting of two com-
ponents. Ambition (initiative, surgency, ambition, and impetuous) and
Sociability (sociable, exhibitionist, and expressive).

4 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY

There is also general agreement about the second dimension. This
factor has been most frequently called Emotional Stability, Stability,
Emotionality, or Neuroticism (Borgatta, 1964; Conley, 1985; Hakel,
1974; John, 1989; Lorr & Manning, 1978; McCrae & Costa, 1985; Noller
et al., 1987; Norman, 1963; Smith, 1967). Common traits associated with
this factor include being anxious, depressed, angry, embarrassed, emo-
tional, worried, and insecure. These two dimensions (Extraversion and
Emotional Stability) represent the “Big Two” described by Eysenck over
40 years ago.

The third dimension has generally been interpreted as Agreeable-
ness or Likability (Borgatta, 1964; Conley, 1985; Goldberg, 1981; Hakel,
1974; Hogan, 1983; John, 1989; McCrae & Costa, 1985; Noller et al.,
1987; Norman, 1963; Smith, 1967; Tupes & Christal, 1961). Others have
labeled it Friendliness (Guilford & Zimmerman, 1949), Social Confor-
mity (Fiske, 1949), Compliance versus Hostile Non-Compliance (Dig-
man & Thkemoto-Chock, 1981), or Love (Peabody & Goldberg, 1989).
Traits associated with this dimension include being courteous, flexible,
trusting, good-natured, cooperative, forgiving, soft-hearted, and toler-
ant.

The fourth dimension has most frequently been called Conscien-
tiousness or Conscience (Botwin & Buss, 1989; Hakel, 1974; John, 1989;
McCrae & Costa, 1985; Noller et al., 1987; Norman, 1963;), although it
has also been called Conformity or Dependability (Fiske, 1949; Hogan,
1983). Because of its relationship to a variety of educational achieve-
ment measures and its association with volition, it has also been called
Will to Achieve or Will (Digman, 1989; Smith, 1967; Wiggins, Black-
burn, & Hackman, 1969), and Work (Peabody & Goldberg, 1989). As
the disparity in labels suggests, there is some disagreement regarding the
essence of this dimension. Some writers (Botwin & Buss, 1989; Fiske,
1949; Hogan, 1983; John, 1989; Noller et al., 1987) have suggested that
Conscientiousness reflects dependability; that is, being careful, thor-
ough, responsible, organized, and planful. Others have suggested that
in addition to these traits, it incorporates volitional variables, such as
hardworking, achievement-oriented, and persevering. Based on the evi-
dence cited by Digman (1990), the preponderance of evidence supports
the definition of conscientiousness as including these volitional aspects
(Bernstein, Garbin, & McClellan, 1983; Borgatta, 1964; Conley, 1985;
Costa & McCrae, 1988; Digman & Inouye, 1986; Digman & Takemoto-
Chock, 1981; Howarth, 1976; Krug & Johns, 1986; Lei & Skinner, 1982;
Lorr & Manning, 1978; McCrae & Costa, 1985, 1987, 1989; Norman,
1963; Peabody & Goldberg, 1989; Smith, 1967).

The last dimension has been the most difficult to identify. It has been
interpreted most frequently as Intellect or Intellectence (Borgatta, 1964;

BARRICK AND MOUNT 5

Digman & Takemoto-Chock, 1981; Hogan, 1983; John, 1989; Peabody
and Goldberg, 1989). It has also been called Openness to Experience
(McCrae & Costa, 1985) or Culture (Hakel, 1974; Norman, 1963). Dig-
man (1990) points out that it is most likely all of these. Itaits commonly
associated with this dimension include being imaginative, cultured, curi-
ous, original, broad-minded, intelligent, and artistically sensitive.

The emergence of the 5-factor model has important implications for
the field of personnel psychology. It illustrates that personality consists
of five relatively independent dimensions which provide a meaningful
taxonomy for studying individual differences. In any field of science, the
availability of such an orderly classification scheme is essential for the
communication and accumulation of empirical findings. For purposes
of this study, we adopted names and definitions similar to those used
by Digman (1990): Extraversion, Emotional Stability, Agreeableness,
Conscientiousness, and Openness to Experience.

Expected Relations Between PersonaUty Dimensions and Job Performance

In the present study, we investigate the validity of the five dimen-
sions of personality for five occupational groups (professionals, police,
managers, sales, and skilled/semi-skilled) and for three types of job per-
formance criteria (job proficiency, training proficiency, and personnel
data) using meta-analytic methods. We also investigate the validity of
the five personality dimensions for objective versus subjective criteria.

We hypothesize that two of the dimensions of personality. Consci-
entiousness and Emotional Stability, will be valid predictors of all job
performance criteria for all jobs. Conscientiousness is expected to be
related to job performance because it assesses personal characteristics
such as persistent, planful, careful, responsible, and hardworking, which
are important attributes for accomplishing work tasks in all jobs. There
is some evidence that in educational settings there are consistent cor-
relations between scores on this dimension and educational achieve-
ment (Digman & Takemoto-Chock, 1981; Smith, 1967). Thus, we ex-
pect that the validity of this dimension will generalize across all occupa-
tional groups and criterion categories. We also expect that the validity
of Emotional Stability will generalize across occupations and criterion
types. Viewing this dimension from its negative pole, we expect that em-
ployees exhibiting neurotic characteristics, such as worry, nervousness,
temperamentalness, high-strungness, and self-pity, will tend to be less
successful than more emotionally stable individuals in all occupations
studied because these traits tend to inhibit rather than facilitate the ac-
complishment of work tasks.

6 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY

We expect that other personality dimensions may be related to job
performance, but only for some occupations or some criteria. For ex-
ample, in those occupations that involve frequent interaction or cooper-
ation with others, we expect that two personality dimensions, Extraver-
sion and Agreeableness, will be valid predictors. These two dimensions
should be predictive of performance criteria for occupations such as
management and sales, but would not be expected to be valid predic-
tors for occupations such as production worker or engineer.

In a similar vein, we expect that Openness to Experience will be a
valid predictor of one of the performance criteria, training proficiency.
This dimension is expected to be related to training proficiency because it
assesses personal characteristics such as curious, broadminded, cultured,
and intelligent, which are attributes associated with positive attitudes
toward learning experiences. We believe that such individuals are more
likely to be motivated to learn upon entry into the training program and,
consequently, are more likely to benefit from the training.

Finally, we investigated a research question of general interest to per-
sonnel psychologists for which we are not testing a specific hypothesis.
The question is whether the validity coefficients for the five personality
dimensions diflfer for two types of criteria, objective and subjective. A
recent meta-analysis by Nathan and Alexander (1988) indicates that, in
general, there is no difference between the magnitude of the validities
for cognitive ability tests obtained for objective and subjective criteria for
clerical jobs. In another study, Schmitt et al. (1984) investigated the va-
lidity of personality measures (across dimensions and occupations) for
different types of criteria, but no definitive conclusions were apparent
from the data. The average validity for the subjective criterion (perfor-
mance ratings) was .206. Validities for three of four objective criteria
were lower (.121 for turnover, .152 for achievement/grades, and .126 for
status change), whereas the validity was higher for wages (.268). Thus,
conclusions regarding whether the validities for personality measures are
higher for objective, compared to subjective, criteria depend to a large
extent on which objective measures are used. Because our study exam-
ines personality using a 5-factor model, we are able to assess whether
dimensions have differential relationships to various objective and sub-
jective criteria.

In summary, the following hypotheses will be tested in this study.
Of the five dimensions of personality. Conscientiousness and Emotional
Stability are expected to be valid predictors of job performance for all
jobs and all criteria because Conscientiousness measures those personal
characteristics that are important for accomplishing work tasks in all
jobs, while Emotional Stability (when viewed from the negative pole)
measures those characteristics that may hinder successful performance.

BARRICK AND MOUNT 7

In contrast, Extraversion and Agreeableness are expected to correlate
with job performance for two occupations, sales and management, be-
cause interpersonal dispositions are likely to be important determinants
of success in those occupations. Finally, Openness to Experience is ex-
pected to correlate with one of the criterion types, training proficiency,
because Openness to Experience appears to assess individuals’ readiness
to participate in learning experiences. In addition, we investigated the
validity of various objective and subjective criteria for the five personality
dimensions.

Method

Literature Review

A literature search was conducted to identify published and unpub-
lished criterion-related validity studies of personality for selection pur-
poses between 1952 and 1988. Three strategies were used to search
the relevant literature. First, a computer search was done of PsycINFO
(1967-1988) and Dissertation Abstracts (1952-1988) in order to find all
references to personality in occupational selection. Second, a manual
search was conducted that consisted of checking the sources cited in the
reference section of literature reviews, articles, and books on this topic,
as well as personality inventory manuals, Buros Tests in Print (volumes 4-
9,1953-1985), and journals that may have included such articles (includ-
ing the Journal of Applied Psychology, Personnel Psychology, Academy of
Management Journal, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro-
cesses/Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, Journal of Man-
agement, Journal of Vocational Behavior, Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, Journal of Personality, and Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology). Finally, personality test publishers and over 60 practition-
ers known to utilize personalify inventories in selection contexts were
contacted by letter, requesting their assistance in sending or locating ad-
ditional published or unpublished validation studies.

Overall, these searches yielded 231 criterion-related validify studies,
117 of which were acceptable for inclusion in this analysis. The remain-
ing 114 studies were excluded for several reasons: 44 reported results
for interest and value inventories only and were excluded because they
did not focus on the validity of personality measures; 24 used composite
scores or, conversely, extracted specific items from difî erent scales and
instruments; 19 reported only significant validity coefficients; 15 used
military or laboratory “subjects”; and 12 either were not selection stud-
ies or provided insufficient information.

8 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY

A total of 162 samples were obtained from the 117 studies. Sample
sizes ranged from 13 to 1,401 (M = 148.11; SD = 185.79), yielding a total
sample of 23,994. Thirty-nine samples were reported in the 1950s, 52 in
the 1960s, 33 in the 1970s, and 38 in the 1980s. Fifty samples (31%) were
collected from unpublished sources, most of which were unpublished
dissertations.

The studies were categorized into five major occupational groupings
and three criterion types. The occupational groups were professionals
(5% of the samples), which consisted of engineers, architects, attorneys,
accountants, teachers, doctors, and ministers; police (13% of the sam-
ples); managers (41% of the samples), which ranged from foremen to
top executives; sales (17% of the samples); and skilledlsemi-skilled (24%
of the samples), which consisted of jobs such as clerical, nurses aides,
farmers, flight attendants, medical assistants, orderlies, airline baggage
handlers, assemblers, telephone operators, grocery clerks, truck drivers,
and production workers.

The three criterion types were fob proficiency (included in 68% of the
samples), training proficiency (12% of the samples), and personnel data
(33% of the samples). It should be noted that in 21 samples, data were
available from two of the three criterion categories, which explains why
the total percent of sample for the three criterion types exceeds 100%.
Similarly, the total sample size on which these analyses are based will be
larger than those for analyses by occupation. Job proficiency measures
primarily included performance ratings (approximately 85% of the mea-
sures) as well as productivity data; training proficiency measures con-
sisted mostly of training performance ratings (approximately 90% of the
measures) in addition to productivity data, such as work sample data and
time to complete training results; and personnel data included data from
employee files, such as salary level, turnover, status change, and tenure.

Key variables of interest in this study were the validity coefficients,
sample sizes, range restriction data for those samples, reliability esti-
mates for the predictors and criteria, the personality scales (and the in-
ventories used), and the types of occupations. A subsample of approx-
imately 25% of the studies was selected to assess interrater agreement
on the coding of the key variables of interest. Agreement was 95% for
these variables and disagreement between coders was resolved by refer-
ring back to the original study.

Scales from all the inventories were classified into the five dimensions
defined earlier (i.e., Extraversion, Emotional Stability, Agreeableness,
Conscientiousness, and Openness to Experience) or a sixth Miscella-
neous dimension. The personality scales were categorized into these di-
mensions by six trained raters. Five of these raters had received Ph.D.s in

BARRICK AND MOUNT 9

psychology (three were practicing consulting psychologists with respon-
sibilities for individual assessment; the other two were professors of psy-
chology and human resources management, respectively, and both had
taught personnel selection courses) and the other taught similar courses
while completing his Ph.D. in human resources management and was
very familiar with the literature on personality. A short training session
was provided to the raters to familiarize them with the rating task and
examples were provided. The description of the five factors provided to
the raters corresponded to those presented by Digman (1990) and as de-
scribed above. Raters were provided a list of the personality scales and
their definitions for each inventory and were instructed to assign each
to the dimension to which it best fit. A sixth category. Miscellaneous,
was used in those cases where the scale could not be assigned clearly
into one of the five categories. If at least five of the six raters agreed
on a dimension, the scale was coded in that dimension. If four of the
six raters agreed and the two authors’ ratings (completed independently
of the raters) agreed with the raters, the scale was coded into that di-
mension. If three or fewer raters agreed, the scale was coded into the
Miscellaneous dimension. At least five of six raters agreed in 68% of the
cases, four of six raters agreed in 23% of the cases, and three or fewer
raters agreed on 9% of the cases. Of the 191 scales, 39 were categorized
as representing Emotional Stability; 32 as Extraversion; 31 as Openness
to Experience; 29 as Agreeableness; 32 as Conscientiousness; 28 as Mis-
cellaneous. (A list of the inventories, their respective scales, and dimen-
sional category assigned are available from the first author.) It should
be noted that an alternative method for assigning the scales would be to
use empirical data, such as factor analyses of inventories or correlations
among scales from different inventories. However, we were unable to
locate sufficient factor analytic studies or correlational data to allow us
to use these approaches because in both cases data was available for only
about half of the variables.

To arrive at an overall validity coefficient for each scale from an in-
ventory, the following decision rules were applied in situations where
more than one validity coefficient was reported from a sample: (a) If an
overall criterion was provided, that coefficient was used and (b) when
multiple criteria were provided, they were assigned to the appropriate
criterion category (job proficiency, training proficiency, or personnel
data). If there were multiple measures from a criterion category, the
coefficients were averaged. However, because our analyses focused on
personality dimensions rather than individual personality scales (from
various inventories), the following decision rules were applied to estab-
lish the validity coefficient for each personality dimension from a sample:
(a) If a personality dimension had only one scale categorized into that

10 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY

dimension for that sample, the overall validify coefficient from that scale
(calculated as previously explained) was used and (b) if multiple scales
were available for a dimension, the coefficients from each of these scales
from that sample were averaged and the resulting average validify coef-
ficient was used in all analyses.

A number of analyses were conducted. The first was an analysis of
the validities for the five personalify dimensions for each occupational
group (across criterion types). The second was an analysis of personalify
dimensions for the three criterion types (across occupations). The final
analysis investigated the validify of the dimensions for objective versus
subjective criteria (across occupations and criterion fypes).

The meta-analytic procedure adopted in this study used the formu-
las available in Hunter and Schmidt (1990)-‘ and corrected the mean and
variance of validify coefficients across studies for artifactual variance due
to sampling error, range restriction, and attenuation due to measure-
ment error. However, because the vast majorify of studies did not report
information on range restriction and measurement error, particularly
predictor reliabilities, it was necessary to use artifact distributions to es-
timate artifactually induced variance on the validify coefficients (Hunter
& Schmidt, 1990).

Because reliabilify coefficients for predictors were only rarely pre-
sented in the validify studies, the distributions were based upon informa-
tion obtained from the inventories’ manuals. The mean of the predictor
reliabilify distribution was .76 (SD = .08). Similarly, because informa-
tion for the criterion reliabilities was available in less than one-third of
the studies, we developed an artifact distribution for criterion reliabili-
ties based on data provided by Hunter, Schmidt, and Judiesch (1990) for
productivify data (with a mean of .92, SD = .05) and Rothstein (1990) for
performance ratings (with a mean of .52, SD = .05). It should be noted,
however, that 30 studies included criteria which were categorized as per-
sonnel data. For these criteria (e.g., turnover, tenure, accidents, wages,
etc.), reliabilify estimates were unknown because no estimates have been
provided in the literature. Therefore, the artifact distributions for crite-
rion reliabilities did not include reliabilify estimates for these criteria.
Thus, for the objective versus subjective analysis, the productivity and
performance rating artifact distributions were used in each analysis, re-
spectively, for each personalify dimension. For all other analyses, the
two criterion distributions were combined (with a mean value of .56, SD
= .10). Finally, the artifact distribution for range restriction data was
based upon those studies that reported both restricted and unrestricted

^All analyses were conducted using a microcomputer program developed by Frank
Schmidt and reported in Hunter and Schmidt, 1990.

BARRICK AND MOUNT 11

Standard deviation data (i.e., from accepted and rejected applicants).
The effects on the mean validities due to range restriction were relatively
small because the mean range restriction was .94 (SD = .05).

As previously stated, the Schmidt-Hunter non-interactive validity
generalization procedure (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990) was applied to the
data for assumed (predictors and criteria) and sample-based artifact dis-
tributions (range restriction). (These distributions are available from the
first author.) However, because the purpose of our study is to enhance
theoretical understanding of the five personality constructs, we present
fully corrected correlations that correct for unreliability in the predictor
as well as the criterion.

Finally, there has been some confusion regarding the use and inter-
pretation of confidence and credibility values in meta-analysis (Whitener,
1990). The confidence interval is centered around the sample-size
weighted mean effects sizes (r, before being corrected for measurement
error or range restriction) and is used to assess the influence of sampling
error on the uncorrected estimate. In contrast, the credibility value is
centered around the estimated true score correlations (generated from
the corrected standard deviation) and is used to assess the influence of
moderators. Our purpose in the present study is to understand the true
score correlations between the personality dimensions and job perfor-
mance criteria for different occupations and to assess the presence of
moderators. Therefore, the focus in this study is on p and the corre-
sponding credibility values.

Results

Analysis by Occupational Group

The number of correlations upon which the meta-analysis is based is
shown in Table 1 for the five personality dimensions, five occupational
types, and three criterion types. It can be seen that the frequencies differ
substantially from cell to cell. For example, the number of correlations
for the job proficiency criterion is generally larger for all personality
dimensions and occupations than for the other criterion types. It can also
be seen that the number of correlations for the management occupation
is greater than for the other occupations. The table also shows that
for some cells there are two or fewer correlations for professionals and
sales for the training proficiency criterion, and for professionals and
police for the personnel data criterion. Consequently, we were unable to

12 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY

TABLE 1
Call Frequencies of Correlations for Personality Dimensions,

Occupational Groups, and Criterion Types

Occupational group

Job proficiency
Professionals
Police
Managers
Sales
Skilled/Semi-skilled

Training proficiency
Professionals
Police
Managers
Sales
Skilled/Semi-skilled

Personnel data
Professionals
Police
Managers
Sales
Skilled/Semi-skilled

Extraversion

4
10
29
16
16

0
6
9
1
3

0
0

21
5
4

Personality dimensions

Emotional
stability

5
12
26
14
15

0
6

10
1
4

0
0

19
4
7

Agree-
ableness

7
8

25
11
17

0
6
9
I
4

0
0

13
4
5

Conscien-
tiousness

6
12
25
17
16

0
5

10
1
3

0
2

17
3
6

Openness to
experience

4
8

19
8

10

0
5
7
I
1

0
0

11
3
5

analyze the data using the 3-way categorization (personalify dimension
by occupational type by criterion fype).

Table 2 presents the results of the meta-analysis for the five person-
alify dimensions across the occupational groups (professionals, police,
managers, sales, and skilled/semi-skilled labor). The first six columns
of the table contain, respectively, the total sample size, the number of
correlation coefficients on which each distribution was based, the un-
corrected (i.e., observed) mean validify, the estimated true correlation
(p), the estimated true residual standard deviation (SDp), and the lower
bound of the 90% credibilify value for each distribution, based on its true
correlation and SDp estimates. The true SDp is the square root of the
variance that was not attributed to the four artifacts (i.e., sampling error
and between-study differences in test unreliabilify, criterion unreliabil-
ify, and degree of range restriction), after correcting for those artifacts.
The last column in Table 2 reports the percentage of observed variance
that was accounted for by the four artifacts.

As shown in Table 2, the correlations for the occupational categories
differed across the five personalify dimensions. Consistent with our hy-
pothesis, the Conscientiousness dimension was a valid predictor for all
occupational groupings. It can be seen that the estimated true score cor-
relations are noticeably larger for Conscientiousness compared to the

BARRICK AND MOUNT

TABLE 2
Meta-Analysis Results for Personality Dimension-Occupation

Combinations (all Criterion Types Included)

13

Occupational group

Extraversion
Professionals
Police
Managers
Sales
Skilled/Semi-Skilled
Mean (across occupations)

Emotional stability

Professionals
Police
Managers
Sales
Skilled/ Semi-Skilled
Mean (across occupations)

Agreeableness

Professionals
Police
Managers
Sales
Skilled/Semi-Skilled
Mean (across occupations)

Conscientiousness
Professionals
Police
Managers
Sales
Skilled/Semi-Skilled

Mean (across occupations)

Openness to experience

Professionals
Police
Managers
Sales
Skilled/Semi-Skilled
Mean (across occupations)

Total N

476
1,496

11,335
2,316
3,888

518
1,697

10,324
2,486
3,694

557
1,437
8,597
2,344
4,585

767
2,045

10,058
2,263
4,588

476
1,364
7,611
1,566
3,219

Number
of r’s

4
16
59
22
23

5
18
55
19
26

7
14
47
16
28

6
19
52
21
25

4
13
37
12
16

Obs
r

– .05
.05
.11
.09
.01
.08

-.07
.06
.05
.04
.05
.05

.01

.06

.05

.00

.04

.04

.11

.13

.13

.09

.12

.13

-.05
.00
.05

-.01
.01
.03

P

– .09
.09
.18
.15
.01
.13

– .13
.10
.08
.07
.12
.08

.02

.10

.10

.00

.06

.07

.20

.22

.22

.23

.21

.22

-.08
.00
.08

-.02
.01
.04

SDp

.05

.00

.13

.16

.08

.11

.04

.00

.09

.19

.10

.10

0
0

.03

.24

.17

.09

.00

.20

.10

.00

.09

.09

.04

.00

.16

.16

.12

.13

90%
C.V

-.03
.09
.01

-.05
-.10
-.01

-.07
.10

-.04
-.18
-.06
-.05

.02

.10

.06
-.31
-.16
-.05

.20
-.03

.09

.23

.09

.10

– .03
.00

-.12
.18

-.15
.13

% Variance
accounted

92
127
48
54
72
69°

92
138
65
38
50
63°

158
121
94
25
37
54°

106
40
64

150
67

70°

94
181
37
46
49
59°

° An unbiased estimate of mean percentage of variance accounted for across meta-
analyses, calculated by taking the reciprocal of the average of reciprocals of individual
predicted to observed variance ratios (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990).

Other personality dimensions and are remarkably consistent across the
five occupational groups (p ranges from .20 to .23).

14 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY

Very little support was found for the hypothesis regarding Emotional
Stability. Compared to the Conscientiousness dimension, the correla-
tions for Emotional Stability are lower (p ranges from -.13 to .12). In
fact, for professionals the relationship was in the opposite direction pre-
dicted (p = -.13).

It was also hypothesized that Extraversion and Agreeableness would
be valid predictors for the two occupations involving interpersonal skills,
managers and sales representatives. This hypothesis was supported for
Extraversion for both occupations (p = .18 and .15, respectively). How-
ever, very little support was obtained for Agreeableness, as p = .10 for
managers and .00 for sales. With respect to the other dimensions, the
remaining true score correlations reported in the table were quite low
(i.e., p = .10 or less).

Analysis by Criteria Type

Table 3 shows the correlation coefficients for the five personality di-
mensions for the three criterion types. Consistent with our hypothe-
sis. Conscientiousness is a valid predictor for each of the three crite-
rion types. As was the case with the occupational analysis in Table 2,
the results for Conscientiousness are quite consistent across the crite-
rion types (p ranges from .20 to .23). As reported, the correlations are
generally higher than for the other personality dimensions. Also consis-
tent with our hypothesis. Openness to Experience predicted the training
proficiency criterion relatively well (p = .25). Interestingly, Extraversion
was also a significant predictor of training proficiency (p = .26). Most
of the remaining correlations for the three criterion types are relatively
small (i.e., p = .10 or less).

Analysis by Objective and Subjective Criteria

Table 4 shows the validity of the five personality dimensions for cri-
teria categorized as objective and subjective. It should be noted that this
analysis is different from that reported in Table 3 because two of the three
criterion types contain some objective and subjective measures. First, it
can be seen that the subjective criteria are used about twice as frequently
as objective criteria. Second, the estimated true score correlations are
generally higher for subjective, compared to objective, criteria. In fact,
only one objective criterion, status change, has true score correlations
equal to or larger than the subjective ratings for four of the personal-
ity dimensions. For the fifth personality dimension. Conscientiousness,
the estimated true correlations for the subjective criteria are higher (p
= .23) than for all objective criteria (p ranges from .12 to .17).

BARRICK AND MOUNT

TABLE 3

Meta-Analysis Results for Personality Dimension and Criteria
(Pooled Across Occupational Groups)

15

Criterion type

Extraversion
Job proficiency
Ti-aining proficiency
Personnel data
Mean (across criteria)

Emotional stability

Job proficiency
Training proficiency
Personnel data
Mean (across criteria)

Agreeableness

Job proficiency
TVaining proficiency
Personnel data
Mean (across criteria)

Conscientiousness
Job proficiency
•Raining proficiency
Personnel data
Mean (across criteria)

Openness to experience

Job proficiency
•Raining proficiency
Personnel data
Mean (across criteria)

Total N

12,396
3,101
6,477

11,635
3,283
5,644

11,526
3,685
4,474

12,893
3,585
6,175

9,454
2,700
3,785

Number
of r’s

89
17
33

87
19
29

80
19
26

92
17
32

55
14
22

Obs
r

.06

.15

.06

08

.04

.04

.05

.05

.04

.06

.08

.04

.13

.13

.11

.13

-.02
.14
.01
.03

P

.10

.26

.11

.13

.07

.07

.09

.08

.06

.10

.14

.07

.23

.23

.20

.22

– .03
.25
.01
.04

SDp

.10

.14

.18

.13

.11
0

.16

.10

.14
0

.11

.10

.10

.15

.10

.10

.04

.16

.15

.09

90%
C.V.

– .03
.08

-.12

– .01

-.07
.07

-.11

-.05

– .12
.10
.00

-.05

.10

.04

.07

.08

.00

.05
– .18
-.02

% Variance
accounted

69
49
33
47°

64
120
38
60°

49
134
59

68°

70
41
71
57°

93
40
44

51°

° An unbiased estimate of mean percentage of variance accounted for across meta-
analyses, calculated by taking the reciprocal of the average of reciprocals of individual
predicted to observed variance ratios (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990).

We conducted additional analyses of the correlation coeflicients by
personality dimensions, criterion types, and occupational subgroups.
Data from these analyses are not reported here (though available upon
request) because for many of the subgroup categories there were too few
validity studies. Overall, however, the results for those subcategories
where data were available do not alter the conclusions reported above.

A key outcome in any meta-analysis of selection studies is the amount
of variation in the validities that is attributed to different situations. For
a majority of the analyses reported in Tables 2, 3, and 4, the percentage
of variance accounted for by the four statistical artifacts (i.e., sampling

16 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY

TABLE 4
Meta-Analysis Results for Personality Dimensions and Objective
and Subjective Criteria (Pooled Across Occupational Groups)

Criterion type

Extraversion

Productivity data
Turnover/Tenure
Status change
Salary
Objective mean (across criteria)
Subjective ratings

Emotional stability

Productivity data
Turnover/Tenure
Status change
Salary
Objective mean (across criteria)
Subjective ratings

Agreeableness

Productivity data
TurnoverATenure
Status change
Salary
Objective mean (across criteria)
Subjective ratings

Conscientiousness
Productivity data
Turnover/Tenure
Status change
Salary
Objective mean (across criteria)
Subjective ratings

Openness to experience

Productivity data
Turnover/Tenure
Status change
Salary
Objective mean (across criteria)
Subjective ratings

Total N

1,774
1,437
4,374

666

12,943

1,436
1,495
3,483

666

12,739

2,082
1,838
2,515

121

12,467

1,639
2,759
2,698

718

14,059

1,060
1,628
1,766

121

10,639

Number
of r’s

12
13
15
4

93

11
13
12
4

95

15
15
9
2

83

14
19
8
5

94

9
12
5
2

62

Obs 90%
r p SDp CV

.07 .10 .03 .06
– . 0 3 – . 0 3 .12 .12

.10 .14 .16 -.06

.04 .06 .08 -.04

.07 .10 .11 -.04

.08 .14 .14 -.05

-.03 -.04 .14 -.14
.01 .02 .17 -.20
.08 .11 .11 -.03

-.01 -.01 0 -.01
.04 .05 .12 -.10
.05 .09 .07 .00

-.03 – .05 .23 .24
.06 .09 0 .09
.09 .13 .13 -.04

-.01 -.02 0 -.02
.04 .05 .14 – .13
.05 .09 .08 -.01

.10 .17 0 .17

.09 .12 .08 .02

.11 .15 .04 .10

.13 .17 .02 .14

.10 .14 .03 .10

.15 .26 .12 .n

.00 .01 0 .01
– . 0 8 – . 1 1 .06 – .03

.09 .12 0 .12

.04 .05 0 .05

.01 .02 .09 -.10

.02 .04 .16 -.16

% Variance
accounted

95
52
31
68
52°
52

45
37
38

181
49″
83

28
129
30

143
48″”
76

176
47
88
97
82°
60

161
80

119
120
113°

42

° An unbiased estimate of mean percentage of variance accounted for across meta-
analyses, calculated by taking the reciprocal of the average of reciprocals of individual
predicted to observed variance ratios (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990).

error and between-study differences in test unreliability, criterion unre-
liability, and degree of range restriction) failed to exceed the 75% rule
(Hunter & Schmidt, 1990). This suggests that differences in correlations
may exist across subpopulations.

BARRICK AND MOUNT 17

Discussion

This study differs from previous studies by using an accepted taxon-
omy to study the relation of personality to job performance criteria. The
results illustrate the benefits of using this classification scheme to com-
municate and accumulate empirical findings. Using this taxonomy, we
were able to show that there are differential relations between the per-
sonality dimensions and occupations and performance criteria.

Before discussing the substantive findings, a comment is in order
regarding the relatively small observed and true score correlations ob-
tained in this study. We would like to re-emphasize that our purpose
was not to determine the overall validity of personality; in fact, we ques-
tion whether such an analysis is meaningful. Rather, the purpose was to
increase our understanding of the way the Big Five personality dimen-
sions relate to selected occupational groups and criterion types.

It is likely that the purpose and methodology used in the present
study, both of which differ from other reviews, may have contributed
to the lower correlations. For example, in the present study, only those
samples that reported zero-order correlations for all scales from an in-
ventory were included in the analysis. Studies were excluded if they re-
ported composite validities or reported only those scales with significant
correlations. Thus, the results for each of the five dimensions are based
on the average of the correlations between personality scales and job
performance criteria. Further, for those studies reporting multiple mea-
sures for each dimension, an average correlation was used in the meta-
analysis, rather than a composite score correlation (which adjusts the
average correlation by the sum of the covariances among the measures
incorporated in the average estimate). Use of the composite score cor-
relation always results in a mean validity estimate larger in size than that
resulting from the average correlation (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990). How-
ever, because intercorrelations among personality scales or dimensions
were generally not reported (even inventory manuals report only a few
intercorrelations), it was not possible to use the composite score corre-
lation in this analysis. A better estimate of the validity of a personality
dimension would be provided by combining all scales measuring a sin-
gle dimension into a predictor composite. Doing this would provide a
better measure of the predictive validity of the construct in question.
Therefore, in interpreting the results of this study, the reader should fo-
cus on understanding which dimensions are the best predictors for spe-
cific occupations and criterion types rather than on the magnitude of the
validities because they are underestimates.

The most significant finding in the study relates to the Conscientious-
ness dimension. It was found to be a consistently valid predictor for all

18 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY

occupational groups studied and for all criterion types. Thus, this as-
pect of personality appears to tap traits which are important to the ac-
complishment of work tasks in all jobs. That is, those individuals who
exhibit traits associated with a strong sense of purpose, obligation, and
persistence generally perform better than those who do not. Similar find-
ings have been reported in educational settings where correlations be-
tween scores on this dimension and educational achievement (Digman &
Takemoto-Chock, 1981; Smith, 1967) and vocational achievement (Take-
moto, 1979) have consistently been reported in the range of .50 to .60.

Further evidence that this dimension is a valid predictor of job per-
formance is found in two studies conducted as part of the U.S. Army Se-
lection and Classification Study (Project A) (Hough, Hanser, & Eaton
1988; McHenry, Hough, Toquam, Hanson, & Ashworth, 1990). Two of
the personality constructs. Achievement Orientation and Dependability,
were found to be valid predictors of job performance measures in both
studies. Although the relationship of the personality constructs investi-
gated by the researchers to the 5-factor taxonomy was not specified, it
appears that these two constructs (Achievement/Achievement Orienta-
tion and Dependability) are aspects of the Conscientiousness dimension
as defined earlier. Achievement taps traits such as planful, organized,
persistent, and hardworking, whereas Dependability assesses traits such
as careful, thorough, and responsible.

An important area of future research suggested by these results is
to further delineate the boundaries of the Conscientiousness dimension.
There is some disagreement among researchers about the precise mean-
ing of this construct. Some define it in terms of responsibility or de-
pendability (e.g., Hogan, 1986), whereas others view it as also including
volitional aspects, such as hardworking, persistent, and achievement-
oriented (e.g., Conley, 1985; Costa & McCrae, 1988; Digman & In-
ouye, 1986; Digman & Takemoto-Chock, 1981; Krug & Johns, 1986;
McCrae & Costa, 1985, 1987, 1989). It is not likely that there will ever
be complete agreement among researchers regarding the content of this
or any of the other dimensions. However, results of recent studies by
researchers in the field of personality psychology, in wfiich scales from
personality inventories are factor analyzed and assessed via the 5-factor
model, may yield valuable insight into the content of the Conscientious-
ness dimension (and the four others as well) (McCrae, 1989).

Another area of research suggested by these results is to investigate
whether measures of Conscientiousness should be incorporated into the-
ories which attempt to account for work performance. For example.
Hunter (1983) has shown that cognitive ability has an indirect effect on
supervisory ratings of performance through its effects on the acquisition
of job knowledge, which in turn impacts work sample performance. In

BARRICK AND MOUNT 19

view of the relatively low correlation between Conscientiousness and
cognitive ability (McCrae, 1989), it seems plausible that this aspect of
personality may account for unique variance in the acquisition of job
knowledge and (therefore) in job performance. Of course, an important
issue is whether the effects of Conscientiousness on job perfonnance are
direct or indirect, or both, and whether the resulting model would gen-
eralize to different occupations. We believe that this is a fertile area for
future research.

Consistent with our hypotheses, Extraversion was a valid predictor
(across the criterion types) for two occupations, managers and sales. For
both types of jobs, interaction with others is a significant portion of the
job. Thus traits such as sociable, gregarious, talkative, assertive, and
active would lead to effective performance in these jobs, whereas these
traits would be less important in jobs such as skilled/semi-skilled (e.g.,
secretaries, orderlies, assemblers, accountants, production workers) and
professionals (e.g., engineers, architects). In both of these cases, how-
ever, the estimated true score correlations are less than .20.

The results with respect to Openness to Experience suggest some
fruitful areas for future research. This personality construct was found
to be a valid predictor of one of the criterion categories, training pro-
ficiency, but not for the other two, job proficiency or personnel data.
One possible explanation of these findings is that individuals who score
high on this dimension (e.g., intelligent, curious, broad-minded, and cul-
tured) are more likely to have positive attitudes toward learning experi-
ences in general. Several researchers have shown that a key component
in the success of training programs is the attitude of the individual when
s/he enters the training program. As Goldstein (1986) states, “… it is
also clear that individuals who are motivated upon entry into the train-
ing program have an advantage from the very beginning” (p. 70). Re-
search by Ryman and Biersner (1975) supports this, as they found that
scores on a scale designed to measure attitudes of trainees prior to the
training (e.g., “If I have trouble during training I will try harder”; “I will
get more from this training than most people”) predicted eventual grad-
uation from a Navy School for Divers. Similarly, Sanders and Vanouzas
(1983) have shown that the attitudes and expectations of the trainees
influence whether or not learning is likely to occur. That is, trainees
who accepted personal responsibility for the learning process and were
willing to participate in discussions, engage in self assessment, and so
forth, were more likely to benefit from the training. Thus, measures
of Openness to Experience may identify which individuals are “training
ready”—those who are most willing to engage in learning experiences—
and, consequently, may be useful in identifying those who are most likely
to benefit from training programs. As a final comment, it should also be

20 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY

recognized that this dimension has the highest correlation (uncorrected,
r = .20 to .30) of any of the personality dimensions with measures of
cognitive ability (McCrae & Costa, 1987). Therefore, it is possible that
Openness to Experience is actually measuring ability to learn as well as
motivation to learn.

Similarly, Extraversion was found to predict the training proficiency
criterion relatively well. Although the relationship with Extraversion was
not hypothesized, in retrospect the findings are not surprising, especially
in light of the types of training programs that were used in these studies.
Most required a high energy level among participants and were highly
interactive, such as assessment centers, police academy training, on-the-
job training for sales and fiight attendants, and so forth. Because Ex-
traversion assesses traits associated with general activity level (talkative,
active, assertive) and sociability, these relationships would be expected.
As Burris (1976) indicates, based on his review of the literature, research
and experience suggest overwhelmingly that learning is more effective
when the learner is active rather than passive. However, it seems logi-
cal that these relations would not exist in training programs that do not
involve social interaction (e.g., lectures, computer assisted instruction,
videotapes). The results for Openness to Experience and Extraversion
suggest that the relation of personality measures to training proficiency
is an important area for future research.

Most of the correlations for Emotional Stability were relatively low.
These findings may be due to a type of range restriction, based on
a “selecting-out” process, which was not accounted for in the present
study. At the extreme, those individuals who are highly neurotic are un-
able to function effectively on their own and, as a result, are not likely
to be in the labor force. More generally, individuals may have “self-
selected out” based on their own interests or perceptions of their emo-
tional stability. Another explanation is that there may not be a linear
relation between Emotional Stability and job performance beyond the
“critically unstable” range. That is, as long as an individual possesses
“enough” Emotional Stability, the predictive value of any differences are
minimized.

Finally, it was interesting to observe that the coefficient for profes-
sionals for this dimension was in a negative direction, suggesting that
individuals who are worrying, nervous, emotional, and high-strung are
better performers in these jobs. It is difficult to explain these results, al-
though it is possible that the causal direction may be such that in some
professional jobs pressures related to high performance cause the indi-
viduals to display neurotic traits. Given that these results are based on
only five samples, however, they should be interpreted cautiously.

BARRICK AND MOUNT 21

The results for Agreeableness suggest that it is not an important pre-
dictor of job performance, even in those jobs containing a large social
component (e.g., sales or management). Such results are in contrast with
the other socially based personality dimension, Extraversion. Thus, it ap-
pears that being courteous, trusting, straight forward, and soft-hearted
has a smaller impact on job performance than being talkative, active, and
assertive.

An issue of general interest to many personnel psychologists is
whether objective measures of job performance result in different va-
lidity results than subjective criteria, particularly because objective and
subjective measures often cannot be treated as substitutes for one an-
other (Heneman, 1986). In general, for the five personality dimensions,
the true score correlations for subjective criteria were larger than for ob-
jective criteria. However, for one objective measure, status change, the
correlations were equal to or larger than subjective measures for four of
the five personality dimensions.

However, for one dimension. Conscientiousness, the correlations for
all objective criteria were smaller than for the subjective ratings. This re-
sult is particularly intriguing because this dimension was found to be the
most predictive personality dimension in this study. A possible explana-
tion for the lower correlations is that the objective criteria are contami-
nated or deficient, or both. An alternative explanation is that the subjec-
tive measures may be susceptible to bias resulting from the individual’s
personality. For example, Hogan (in press) suggests that one meaning
of personality refers to a person’s social reputation; that is to the man-
ner in which he or she is perceived by friends, co-workers, and super-
visors. Viewed in this way, personality concerns the amount of esteem,
regard, and status accorded by his or her social groups. Thus, according
to Hogan, reputations summarize what individuals say about a person’s
past behaviors and may be used to forecast future performance or, at
least, what others are likely to say about a person’s future behavior. The
higher correlations for the subjective criteria in the present study suggest
that one’s reputation may infiuence judgments of performance.

The results of the present study have implications for both research
and practice in personnel selection. From a practitioner’s standpoint,
the results suggest that if the purpose is to predict job performance based
on an individual’s personality, then those measures associated with Con-
scientiousness are most likely to be valid predictors for all jobs. In fact,
it is difficult to conceive of a job in which the traits associated with the

22 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY

Conscientiousness dimension would not contribute to job success. In-
terestingly, this dimension, which measures traits such as planful, orga-
nized, hardworking, persistent, and achievement oriented, has been la-
beled Work by Peabody and Goldberg (1989) in recognition that these
personality traits are closely related to the performance of work tasks.

Of course this does not preclude the possibility that some measures
from other personality dimensions may predict job performance. For ex-
ample, Hogan and Hogan (1989) developed and validated a personality
instrument called Employee Reliability, which was composed of relevant
measures from the five personality dimensions studied in this analysis.
A large component of this instrument was associated with dependability
and carefulness, traits which are representative of the Conscientiousness
dimension in the present study. Thus, their finding that the Employee
Reliability instrument was a valid predictor of job performance crite-
ria (in addition to counterproductive work behaviors) is consistent with
the results of the present study. Taken together, these results further
strengthen the conclusion that the most predictive measures of personal-
ity are those that emphasize traits associated with the Conscientiousness
dimension. (For another example, the reader is directed to the Work
Orientation Scale developed by Gough (1985), based on items from mul-
tiple scales of the CPI).

In retrospect, it is not surprising that the overall validity of person-
ality measures has been found to be relatively low. As one example, the
MMPI is often seen as the prototypical personality inventory. However,
a fact that cannot be overlooked is that the MMPI was not designed to
predict job performance in normal populations. Thus, findings that the
MMPI is a relatively poor predictor of job performance would be ex-
pected, based on the results of this study, because most scales on the
MMPI measure Emotional Stability and none measure Conscientious-
ness directly (based on the classifications made by the raters in this study
and based on factor analyses of the MMPI by Johnson, Null, Butcher, &
Johnson, 1984). Thus, the results of the present study are also useful in
explaining why some personality inventories are likely to be better pre-
dictors of job performance than others.

Of interest to those in the training and development field are the
findings that two of the personality dimensions. Openness to Experience
and Extraversion, are related to performance in training programs. Very
little research has investigated the relation of individual measures of per-
sonality to measures of training readiness and training success. Perhaps
future research and practice in the training and development field will
be stimulated by the availability of a classification scheme for organizing
individual differences in personality.

BARRICK AND MOUNT 23

In summary, in order for any fleld of science to advance, it is nec-
essary to have an accepted classiflcation scheme for accumulating and
categorizing empirical flndings. We believe that the robustness of the
5-factor model provides a meaningful framework for formulating and
testing hypotheses relating individual differences in personality to a wide
range of criteria in personnel psychology, especially in the subflelds of
personnel selection, performance appraisal, and training and develop-
ment.

REFERENCES

Bernstein IH, Garbin CP, McClellan PG. (1983). A confirmatory factoring of the California
Psychological Inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 43, 687-691.

Bond MH, Nakazato HS, Shiraishi D. (1975). Universality and distinctiveness in dimen-
sions of Japanese person perception. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 6, 346-
355.

Borgatta EE (1964). The structure of personality characteristics. Behavioral Science, 12,
8-17.

Botwin MD, Buss DM. (1989). Structure of act-report data: Is the five-factor model of
personality recaptured? Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 56, 988-1001.

Briggs SR. (1989). The optimal level of measurement for personality constructs. In Buss
DM, Cantor N (Eds.), Personality Psychology: Recent trends and emerging directions.
New York: Springer-Verlag.

Burris RW. (1976). Human Learning. In Dunnette MD. (Ed.), Handbook of Industrial and
Organizational Psychology. Chicago, Rand McNally.

Cattell RB. (1943). The description of personality: basic traits resolved into clusters.
Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 38, 476-506.

Cattell RB. (1946). The description and measurement of personality. Yonkers, NY: World
Book.

Cattell RB. (1947). Confirmation and clarification of primary personality factors. Psy-
chometrika, 12,197-220.

Cattell RB. (1948). The primary personality factors in women compared with those in
men. British Journal of Psychology, 1,114-130.

Conley JJ. (1985). Longitudinal stability of personality traits: A multitrait-multimethod-
multioccasion analysis. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 49,1266-1282.

Costa PT Jr., McCrae RR. (1988). From catalog to classification: Murray’s needs and the
five-factor model. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 55, 258-265.

Digman JM. (1989). Five robust trait dimensions: Development, stability, and utility.
Journal of Personality, 57,195-214.

Digman JM. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. Annual
Review of Psychology, 41,417-440.

Digman JM, Inouye J. (1986). Further specification of the five robust factors of personality.
Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 50, 116-123.

Digman JM, Takemoto-Chock NK. (1981). Factors in the natural language of personal-
ity: Re-Analysis, comparison, and interpretation of six major studies. Multivariate
Behavioral Research, 16,149-170.

Fiske DW. (1949). Consistency of the factorial structures of personality ratings from dif-
ferent sources. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 44, 329-344.

Ghiselli EE. (1973). The validity of aptitude tests in personnel selection, PERSONNEL
PSYCHOLOGY, 26, 4 6 1 ^ 7 7 .

24 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY

Goldberg LR. (1981). Language and individual differences: The search for universals in
personality lexicons. In Wheeler L. (Ed.), Review of PersonaUty and Social Psychol-
ogy (Vol. 2, pp. 141-166). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.

Goldstein IL. (1986). Trainingin Organizations: Needs Assessment, Development, and Eval-
uation. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.

Gough HG. (1985). A work orientation scale for the California Psychological Inventory.
Journal of Applied Psychotogy, 70,505-513.

Guilford JP, Zimmerman WS. (1949). The Guilford-Zimmerman Temperament Survey.
Beverly Hills, CA: Sheridan Supply.

Guion RM, Gottier RF. (1965). Validity of personality measures in personnel selection.
PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY, 18, 135-164.

Hakel MD. (1974). Normative personality factors recovered from ratings of personality
descriptors: The beholder’s eye. PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY, 27,409-421.

Heneman RL. (1986). The relationship between supervisory ratings and results-oriented
measures of performance: A meta-analysis. PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY, 59, 811-826.

Hogan R. (1983). A socioanalytic theory of personality. In Page MM. (Ed.), Personality-
current theory & research: Nebraska symposium on motivation. Lincoln, NE: Univer-
sity of Nebraska Press.

Hogan R. (1986). Manual for the Hogan Personality Inventory. Minneapolis: National
Computer Systems.

Hogan R. (In press). Personality and personality measurement. In Dunnette MD. (Ed.),
Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Psychologists Press.

Hogan J, Hogan R. (1989). How to measure reliability. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74,
273-279.

Hough LM, Hanser LM, Eaton NK. (1988). Literature review: Utility of temperament,
biodata, and interest assessment for predicting job peiformance. Alexandria, VA: U.S.
Army, Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences (ARI research
note 88-02).

Howarth E. (1976). Were Cattell’s ‘personality sphere’ factors correctly identified in the
first instance? British Journal of Psychology, 67, 213-230.

Hunter JE. (1983). A causal analysis of cognitive ability, job knowledge, job performance,
and supervisory ratings. In Landy F, Zedeck S, Cleveland J (Eds.), Performance
Measurement and Theory (pp. 257-266). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum.

Hunter JE, Schmidt FL. (1990). Methods of meta-analysis: correcting error and bias in
research findings. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.

Hunter JE, Schmidt FL, Judiesch MK. (1990). Individual differences in output as a func-
tion of job complexity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75,28^2.

John OP. (1989). Towards a taxonomy of personality descriptors. In Buss DM, Cantor
N (Eds.), Personality psychology: Recent trends and emerging directions. New York:
Springer-Verlag.

Johnson JH, Null C, Butcher JN, Johnson KN. (1984). Replicated item level factor analysis
of the full MMPI. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47,105-114.

Krug SE, Johns EF. (1986). A large scale cross-validation of second-order personality
structure defined by the 16PF Psychological Reporis, 59, 683-693.

Lei H, Skinner HA. (1982). What difference does language make? Structural analysis of
the personality research form. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 17, 33-46.

Livneh H, Livneh C. (1989). The five-factor model of personality: Is evidence for its cross-
media premature? Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 75-80.

Locke EA, Hulin CL. (1962). A review and evaluation of the validity studies of activity
vector analysis, PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY, 15, 25-42.

BARRICK AND MOUNT 25

Lorr M, Manning TT. (1978). Higher-order personality factors of the ISI. Multivariate
Behavioral Research, 13,3-7.

Lorr M, Youniss RP. (1973). An inventory of interpersonal style. Joumal of Personality
Assessment, 37,165-173.

McCrae RR. (1989). Why I advocate the five-factor model: Joint factor analyses of the
NEO-PI with other instruments. In Buss DM, Cantor N (Eds.), Personality psychol-
ogy: Recent trends and emerging directions. New York: Springer-Verlag.

McCrae RR, Costa PT Jr. (1985). Updating Norman’s “adequate taxonomy”: Intelligence
and personality dimensions in natural language and in questionnaires. Joumal of
Personality & Social Psychology, 49, 710-721.

McCrae RR, Costa PT Jr. (1987). Validation of the five-factor model of personality across
instruments and observers. Joumal of Personality & Social Psychology, 52,81-90.

McCrae RR, Costa PT Jr. (1989). The structure of interpersonal traits: Wiggin’s cir-
cumplex and the five-factor model. Joumal of Personality & Social Psychology, 56,
586-595.

McDougall W. (1932). Of the words character and personality. Character Personality, 1,
3-16.

McHenry JJ, Hough LM, Toquam JL, Hanson MA, Ashworth S. (1990). Project A validity
results: The relationship between predictor and criterion domains, PERSONNEL
PSYCHOLOGY, 43, 335-367.

Nathan BR, Alexander RA. (1988). A comparison of criteria for test validation: A meta-
analytic investigation, PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY, 41,517-535.

Noller P, Law H, Comrey AL. (1987). Cattell, Comrey, and Eysenck personality factors
compared: More evidence for the five robust factors? Joumal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 53, 775-782.

Norman WX (1963). Toward an adequate taxonomy of personality attributes: Replicated
factor structure in peer nomination personality ratings. Joumal of Abnormal &
Social Psychology, 66, 574-583.

Norman WT, Goldberg LR. (1966). Raters, ratees, and randomness in personality struc-
ture. Joumal of Personality & Social Psychology, 4, 681-691.

Peabody D, Goldberg LR. (1989). Some determinants of factor structures from personality-
trait descriptors. Joumal of Personality & Social Psychology, 57,552-567.

Reilly RR, Chao GT. (1982). Validity and fairness of some alternative employee selection
procedures, PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY, i5,1-62.

Ryman DH, Biersner RJ. (1975). Attitudes predictive of training success, PERSONNEL
PSYCHOLOGY, 28, 181-188.

Sanders P, Vanouzas JN. (1983). Socialization to learning. Training and Development
Joumal, 37,14-21.

Schmitt N, Gooding RZ, Noe RA, Kirsch M. (1984). Meta-analyses of validity studies
published between 1964 and 1982 and the investigation of study characteristics.
PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY, 37, 407-422.

Smith GM. (1967). Usefulness of peer ratings of personality in educational research.
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 27, 967-984.

Takemoto NK. (1979). The prediction of occupational choice from childhood and adolescent
antecedents. Unpublished masters thesis. University of Hawaii, Honolulu, HI.

Tupes EC. (1957). Personality traits related to effectiveness of junior and senior Air Force
officers (USAF Personnel Training Research, No. 57-125). Lackland Airforce Base,
TX: Aeronautical Systems Division, Personnel Laboratory.

Tupes EC, Christal RE. (1961, May). Recurrent personality factors based on trait ratings
(ASD-TR-61-97). Lackland Air Force Base, TX: Aeronautical Systems Division,
Personnel Laboratory.

26 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY

Waller NG, Ben-Porath YS. (1987). Is it time for clinical psychology to embrace the five-
factor model of personality? American Psychologist, 42,887-889.

Watson D. (1989). Strangers’ ratings of the five robust personality factors: Evidence of a
surprising convergence with self-report. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology,
57,120-128.

Whitener EM. (1990). Confusion of confidence intervals and credibility intervals in Meta-
Analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 315-321.

Wiggins N, Blackburn M, Hackman JR. (1969). The prediction of first-year success in
psychology: Peer ratings. Journal of Educational Research, 63, 81-85.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
1988, Vol, 54, No. 6, 1063-1070

Copyright 1988 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
0022-3514/88/Í00.75

Th
is

do
cu

m
en

t i
s c

op
yr

ig
ht

ed
b

y
th

e
A

m
er

ic
an

P
sy

ch
ol

og
ic

al
A

ss
oc

ia
tio

n
or

o
ne

o
f i

ts
al

lie
d

pu
bl

ish
er

s.
Th

is
ar

tic
le

is
in

te
nd

ed
so

le
ly

fo
r t

he
p

er
so

na
l u

se
o

f t
he

in
di

vi
du

al
u

se
r a

nd
is

n
ot

to
b

e
di

ss
em

in
at

ed
b

ro
ad

ly
.

Development and Validation of Brief Measures of Positive
and Negative Affect: The PANAS Scales

David Watson and Lee Anna Clark
Southern Methodist University

Auke Tellegen
University of Minnesota

in recent studies of the structure of affect, positive and negative affect have consistently emerged as
two dominant and relatively independent dimensions. A number of mood scales have been created
to measure these factors; however, many existing measures are inadequate, showing low reliability
or poor convergent or discriminant validity. To fill the need for reliable and valid Positive Affect and
Negative Affect scales that are also brief and easy to administer, we developed two 10-item mood
scales that comprise the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). The scales are shown to be
highly internally consistent, largely uncorrelated, and stable at appropriate levels over a 2-month
time period. Normative data and factorial and external evidence of convergent and discriminant
validity for the scales are also presented.

Two dominant dimensions consistently emerge in studies of
affective structure, both in the United States and in a number
of other cultures. They appear as the first two factors in factor
analyses of self-rated mood and as the first two dimensions in
multidimensional scalings of facial expressions or mood terms
(Diener, Larsen, Levine, & Emmons, 1985; Russell, 1980,
1983; Stone, 1981; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1984; Zevon &
Tellegen, 1982).

Watson and Tellegen (1985) have summarized the relevant
evidence and presented a basic, consensual two-factor model.
Whereas some investigators work with the unrotated dimen­
sions (typically labeled pleasantness-unpleasantness and
arousal), the varimax-rotated factors—usually called Positive
Affect and Negative Affect—have been used more extensively in
the self-report mood literature; they are the focus of this article.
Although the terms Positive Affect and Negative Affect might
suggest that these two mood factors are opposites (that is,
strongly negatively correlated), they have in fact emerged as
highly distinctive dimensions that can be meaningfully repre­
sented as orthogonal dimensions in factor analytic studies of
affect.

Briefly, Positive Affect (PA) reflects the extent to which a per­
son feels enthusiastic, active, and alert. High PA is a state of
high energy, full concentration, and pleasurable engagement,
whereas low PA is characterized by sadness and lethargy. In con­
trast, Negative Affect (NA) is a general dimension of subjective
distress and unpleasurable engagement that subsumes a variety
of aversive mood states, including anger, contempt, disgust,
guilt, fear, and nervousness, with low NA being a state of calm-

We wish to thank Lisa Binz, Sondra Brumbelow, Richard Cole, Mary
Dieffenwierth, Robert Folger, Jay Leeka, Curt McIntyre, James Pen-
nebaker, and Karen Schneider for their help in collecting the data re­
ported in this article.

Correspondence should be addressed to David Watson, Department
of Psychology, Southern Methodist University, Dallas, Texas, 75275.

ness and serenity. These two factors represent affective state di­
mensions, but Tellegen (1985; see also Watson & Clark, 1984)
has demonstrated that they are related to corresponding affec­
tive trait dimensions of positive and negative emotionality (indi­
vidual differences in positive and negative emotional reactivity).
Trait PA and NA roughly correspond to the dominant personal­
ity factors of extraversion and anxiety/neuroticism, respectively
(Tellegen, 1985; Watson & Clark, 1984). Drawing on these and
other findings, Tellegen has linked trait NA and PA, respectively,
to psychobiological and psychodynamic constructs of sensitiv­
ity to signals of reward and punishment. He has also suggested
that low PA and high NA (both state and trait) are major distin­
guishing features of depression and anxiety, respectively (Tel­
legen, 1985; see also Hall, 1977).

Numerous PA and NA scales have been developed and stud­
ied in a variety of research areas. Generally speaking, the find­
ings from these studies indicate that the two mood factors relate
to different classes of variables. NA—but not PA—is related to
self-reported stress and (poor) coping (Clark & Watson, 1986;
Kanner, Coyne, Schaefer, & Lazarus, 1981; Wills, 1986), health
complaints (Beiser, 1974; Bradburn, 1969; Tessier & Mechanic,
1978; Watson & Pennebaker, in press), and frequency of un­
pleasant events (Stone, 1981; Warr, Barter, & Brownbridge,
1983). In contrast, PA—but not NA—is related to social activ­
ity and satisfaction and to the frequency of pleasant events
(Beiser, 1974; Bradburn, 1969; Clark & Watson, 1986, 1988;
Watson, 1988).

Anomalous and inconsistent findings have also been re­
ported, however. For example, whereas most studies have found
these NA and PA scales to have low or nonsignificant corre­
lations with one another (e.g., Clark & Watson, 1986, 1988;
Harding, 1982; Moriwaki, 1974; Warr, 1978; Wills, 1986), oth­
ers have found them to be substantially related (Brenner, 1975;
Diener & Emmons, 1984; Kammann, Christie, Irwin, &
Dixon, 1979). There are many possible explanations for such
inconsistencies (e.g., see Diener & Emmons, 1984), but one that
must be considered concerns the various scales themselves. It

1063

Th
is

do
cu

m
en

t i
s c

op
yr

ig
ht

ed
b

y
th

e
A

m
er

ic
an

P
sy

ch
ol

og
ic

al
A

ss
oc

ia
tio

n
or

o
ne

o
f i

ts
al

lie
d

pu
bl

ish
er

s.
Th

is
ar

tic
le

is
in

te
nd

ed
so

le
ly

fo
r t

he
p

er
so

na
l u

se
o

f t
he

in
di

vi
du

al
u

se
r a

nd
is

n
ot

to
b

e
di

ss
em

in
at

ed
b

ro
ad

ly
.

1064 D. WATSON, L. CLARK,

may be, for example, that some scales are simply better, purer
measures of the underlying factors than are others. Watson (in
press) reported evidence supporting this idea. He found that
some scale pairs (such as those used by Diener and his associates
in a number of studies; e.g., Diener & Emmons, 1984; Diener &
Iran-Nejad, 1986; Diener et al., 1985) yield consistently higher
NA-PA correlations than do others (such as our own scales, to
be described shortly).

More generally, one must question the reliability and validity
of many of these measures. Some mood scales have been devel­
oped through factor analysis (e.g., Stone, 1981), but others have
been constructed on a purely ad hoc basis with no supporting
reliability or validity data (e.g., McAdams & Constantian,
1983). Watson (in press) analyzed the psychometric properties
of several popular measures and found many of them to be
wanting, at least for use in student populations. For example,
Bradburn’s (1969) widely used NA and PA scales were unreli­
able (coefficient « = .52 for NA, .54 for PA) and only moder­
ately related to other measures of the same factor (for NA, the
convergent correlations ranged from .39 to .52; for PA, they
ranged from .41 to .53). The short PA and NA scales used by
Stone and his colleagues (Hedges, Jandorf, & Stone, 1985;
Stone, 1987; Stone, Hedges, Neale, & Satin, 1985) were also
unreliable (in two samples, the NA scale had coefficient as of
.48 and .52, whereas the PA scale had corresponding values of
.64 and .70).

Clearly there is a need for reliable and valid PA and NA scales
that are also brief and easy to administer. In this article we de­
scribe the development of such scales, the 10-item NA and PA
scales that comprise the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule
(PANAS), and present reliability and validity evidence to sup­
port their use.

Development of the PANAS Scales

Much of our previous mood research has been concerned
with identifying these dominant dimensions of affect and clari­
fying their nature (Clark & Watson, 1986, 1988; Tellegen, 1985;
Watson, in press; Watson & Clark, 1984; Watson et al., 1984;
Watson & Tellegen, 1985; Zevon & Tellegen, 1982). To have a
broad and representative sample of mood descriptors, we have
used questionnaires that contained a large number (57-65) of
mood terms. Once the basic NA and PA factors were clearly
identified, however, we wanted to measure them more simply
and economically. We therefore turned our attention to the de­
velopment of brief PA and NA scales.

Our greatest concern was to select terms that were relatively
pure markers of either PA or NA; that is, terms that had a sub­
stantial loading on one factor but a near-zero loading on the
other. As a starting point, we used the 60 terms included in the
factor analyses reported by Zevon and Tellegen (1982). This
sample of descriptors was constructed by selecting three terms
from each of 20 content categories; for example, the terms
guilty, ashamed, and blameworthy comprise the guilty category
(see Zevon & Tellegen, 1982, Table 1 ). The categories were iden­
tified through a principal-components analysis of content sort­
ings of a large sample of descriptors and provide a comprehen­
sive sample of the affective lexicon.

From this list we selected those terms that had an average
loading of .40 or greater on the relevant factor across both the

AND A. TELLEGEN

R- and P-analyses reported in Zevon & Tellegen ( 1982). Twenty
PA markers and 30 NA markers met this initial criterion. How­
ever, as noted previously, we were also concerned that the terms
not have strong secondary loadings on the other factor. We
therefore specified that a term could not have a secondary load­
ing of |.25| or greater in either analysis. This reduced the pool
of candidate descriptors to 12 for PA and 25 for NA.

Preliminary reliability analyses convinced us that 10 terms
were sufficient for the PANAS PA scale; we therefore dropped 2
terms (delightedand healthy) that had relatively high secondary
loadings on NA. This yielded the final list of 10 descriptors for
the PA scale: attentive, interested, alert, excited, enthusiastic,
inspired, proud, determined, strong and active.

The 25 NA candidate terms included all 3 terms from seven
of the content categories (distressed, angry, contempt, revul­
sion, fearful, guilty, and jittery) and 2 from each of two others
(rejected and angry at self). Because we wanted to tap a broad
range of content, we constructed a preliminary 14-item scale
that included 2 terms from each of the seven complete triads.
We found, however, that the contempt and revulsion terms did
not significantly enhance the reliability and validity of the scale.
Moreover, these terms were less salient to our subjects and were
occasionally left unanswered. We therefore settled on a final 10-
item version that consisted of 2 terms from each of the other five
triads: distressed, upset (distressed); hostile, irritable (angry);
scared, afraid (fearful); ashamed, guilty (guilty); and nervous,
jittery (jittery). The final version of PANAS is given in the Ap­
pendix.

Reliability and Validity of the PANAS Scales

Subjects and Measures

The basic psychometric data were gathered primarily from
undergraduates enrolled in various psychology courses at
Southern Methodist University (SMU), a private southwestern
university. The students participated in return for extra course
credit. In addition, groups of SMU employees completed ques­
tionnaires asking how they felt “during the past few weeks”
{n = 164) and “during the past few days” (n = 50). A sample of
53 adults not affiliated with SMU also filled out a mood form
with “today” time instructions. Preliminary analyses revealed
no systematic differences between student and nonstudent re­
sponses, and they have been combined in all analyses. Neverthe­
less, because most of our data were collected from college stu­
dents, it is important to establish that the PANAS scales also
work reasonably well in adult and clinical samples. We briefly
address this issue in a later section.

The mood questionnaire consisted of a single раде with the
60 Zevon and Tellegen (1982) descriptors arrayed in various
orders. The subjects were asked to rate on a 5-point scale the
extent to which they had experienced each mood state during
a specified time frame. The points of the scale were labeled very
slightly or not at all, a little, moderately, quite a bit, and very
much, respectively. The PANAS terms were randomly distrib­
uted throughout the questionnaire. It is important to note that
we have since used the 20 PANAS descriptors without these ad­
ditional terms and obtained essentially identical results (Clark
& Watson, 1986; Watson, 1988).

We obtained ratings with seven different temporal instruc-

DEVELOPMENT AND VALIDATION OF THE PANAS SCALES 1065

Th
is

do
cu

m
en

t i
s c

op
yr

ig
ht

ed
b

y
th

e
A

m
er

ic
an

P
sy

ch
ol

og
ic

al
A

ss
oc

ia
tio

n
or

o
ne

o
f i

ts
al

lie
d

pu
bl

ish
er

s.
Th

is
ar

tic
le

is
in

te
nd

ed
so

le
ly

fo
r t

he
p

er
so

na
l u

se
o

f t
he

in
di

vi
du

al
u

se
r a

nd
is

n
ot

to
b

e
di

ss
em

in
at

ed
b

ro
ad

ly
.

Table 1
Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) Scale Means
and Standard Deviations for Each Rated Time Frame

Time
instructions n

PANAS PA
Scale

PANAS NA
Scale

Μ SD Μ SD

Moment 660 29.7 7.9 14.8 5.4
Today 657 29.1 8.3 16.3 6.4
Past few days 1,002 33.3 7.2 17.4 6.2
Past few weeks 586 32.0 7.0 19.5 7.0
Year 649 36.2 6.3 22.1 6.4
General 663 35.0 6.4 18.1 5.9

Note. PA = Positive Affect. NA = Negative Affect.

tions. Subjects were asked to rate how they felt (a) “right now
(that is, at the present moment)” {moment instructions); (b)
“today” (today); (c) “during the past few days” (past few days);
(d) “during the past week” (week); (e) “during the past few
weeks” (past few weeks); (f) “during the past year” (year); and
(g) “in general, that is, on the average” (general). For six of these
time frames, we collected data on large samples to be used for
normative, internal consistency, and factor analyses. The ns are
660 (moment), 657 (today), 1,002 (past few days), 586 (past few
weeks), 649 (year), and 663 (general). These samples are largely
but not completely independent: Some subjects completed
mood forms involving two or more different temporal instruc­
tions; such multiple ratings were always spaced at least 1 week
apart. In addition, a subset of these subjects (n – 101) com­
pleted ratings on all seven time frames on two different occa­
sions, providing retest data.

Normative and Reliability Data

Basic scale data. Table 1 presents basic descriptive data on
the PANAS PA and NA scales for the various time instructions.
Given the large sample sizes, these provide reasonably good col­
lege student norms. In our data, we have not found any large or
consistent sex differences, so the data are collapsed across sex.
Nevertheless, it seems advisable to test for sex differences in any
new (especially nonstudent) sample.

Inspecting Table 1, one sees that subjects report more PA
than NA, regardless of the time frame. Moreover, mean scores
on both scales tend to increase as the rated time frame length­
ens. This pattern is expectable: As the rated time period in­
creases, the probability that a subject will have experienced a
significant amount of a given affect also increases.

The PANAS scale intercorrelations and internal consistency
reliabilities (Cronbach’s coefficient a) are reported in Table 2.
The alpha reliabilities are all acceptably high, ranging from .86
to .90 for PA and from .84 to .87 for NA. The reliability of the
scales is clearly unaffected by the time instructions used.

The correlation between the NA and PA scales is invariably
low, ranging from —.12 to —.23; thus, the two scales share ap­
proximately 1 % to 5% of their variance. These discriminant val­
ues indicate quasi-independence, an attractive feature for many
purposes, and are substantially lower than those of many other
short PA and NA scales (see Watson, in press). Interestingly,

our PA-NA correlation was unaffected by the rated time frame,
whereas Diener and Emmons (1984) found that the correlation
between their PA and NA scales decreased as the rated time
frame lengthened. However, this discrepancy is beyond the
scope of our article; see Watson (in press) for a detailed discus­
sion of the effects of different temporal instructions on various
mood scales.

Test-relest reliability. As noted previously, 101 SMU under­
graduates filled out PANAS ratings for each of the seven time
frames on two different occasions. The mood ratings were col­
lected at weekly intervals. The first set of ratings was collected
during Weeks 1-7 of the fall 1986 semester in the following or­
der: year, past few days, today, past few weeks, general, moment,
and week. Then, following a 1-week break, the PANAS scales
were readministered during Weeks 9-15 in the same sequence.
Thus, each scale was retested after an 8-week interval.

These reliability data are shown in Table 3. The NA and PA
stability values were first compared at each rated time frame
and no significant differences were found (p > .05, 2-tailed t
test). Multiple comparisons were then made across the time
frames for each affect separately (p < .002, Bonferroni cor­
rected for 21 comparisons). Not surprisingly, the retest stability
tends to increase as the rated time frame lengthens. Ratings of
longer time periods, such as how one has felt during the past
few weeks or the past year, are implicit aggregations. In a sense,
subjects average their responses over a longer time frame and
hence over more occasions. Thus, these data replicate the fre­
quent finding that stability rises with increasing temporal ag­
gregation (e.g., Diener & Larsen, 1984; Epstein, 1979). The sta­
bility coefficients of the general ratings are high enough to sug­
gest that they may in fact be used as trait measures of affect.

It is also noteworthy that the PANAS scales exhibit a signifi­
cant level of stability in every time frame, even in the moment
ratings. These results are also consistent with earlier findings
(e.g., Watson & Clark, 1984, Table 8) and reflect the strong dis­
positional component of affect. That is, even momentary
moods are, to a certain extent, reflections of one’s general
affective level (Costa & McCrae, 1980; Watson & Clark, 1984).

Generalizability to nonstudent samples. Our largest nonstu­
dent sample consisted of 164 SMU employees who rated how
they had felt during the past few weeks. A separate analysis of
this sample yielded results comparable with the values listed in

Alpha reliabilities

Table 2
Internal Consistency Reliabilities (Coefficient A Ipha)
and Scale Intercorrelations

Time
instructions n

PANAS PA
scale

PANAS NA
scale

PA-NA
intercor­

relation

Moment 660 .89 .85 -.15
Today 657 .90 .87 -.12
Past few days 1,002 .88 .85 -.22
Past few weeks 586 .87 .87 -.22
Year 649 .86 .84 -.23
General 663 .88 .87 -.17

Note. PANAS = Positive and Negative Affect Schedule. PA = Positive
Affect. NA = Negative Affect.

1066 D. WATSON, L. CLARK, AND A. TELLEGEN

Tesi-Retest Reiiabilities of the Positive and Negative
Table 3

Affect Schedule (PANAS) Scales (8- Week Retest Interval)

Time
instructions

PANAS PA
scale

PANAS NA
scale

Moment .54Bb .45”
Today ,47b .39b

Past few days .48” .42b

Past week .47” .47”
Past few weeks ,58“b ,48b

Year ,63“b .60“b

General .68“ .71“

Note. n = 101. Coefficients not sharing the same superscript are differ­
ent at p < .05 (two-tailed, Bonferroni corrected for multiple compari­
sons). PA = Positive Affect. NA = Negative Affect. Significance tests are
computed separately for each scale. See text for further details.

Th
is

do
cu

m
en

t i
s c

op
yr

ig
ht

ed
b

y
th

e
A

m
er

ic
an

P
sy

ch
ol

og
ic

al
A

ss
oc

ia
tio

n
or

o
ne

o
f i

ts
al

lie
d

pu
bl

ish
er

s.
Th

is
ar

tic
le

is
in

te
nd

ed
so

le
ly

fo
r t

he
p

er
so

na
l u

se
o

f t
he

in
di

vi
du

al
u

se
r a

nd
is

n
ot

to
b

e
di

ss
em

in
at

ed
b

ro
ad

ly
.

Table 2. Specifically, the alpha reliabilities of the PANAS PA
and NA scales were .86 and .87, respectively, and the correlation
between the scales was —.09. Given these data, we believe that
the PANAS scales will provide useful information in adult sam­
ples as well, although further data are desirable to establish this
fully.

We have also collected data on a small (n = 61) psychiatric
inpatient sample using the general instructions. Again, the PA­
NAS scales were reliable (for PA, a = .85; for NA, a = .91) and
only moderately intercorrelated with one another (r = -.27).
Given the small sample size, these data cannot be considered
definitive, but they are encouraging and suggest that the PANAS
scales retain their reliability and quasi-independence in clinical
samples. In addition, all but four of the patients retook the mea­
sure after a 1-week interval, and the resulting stability analyses
yielded high test-retest reliabilities: .81 for NA and .79 for PA.
Finally, consistent with previous studies (Watson & Clark,
1984), we found significant group differences for NA, with the
patients considerably higher (Μ = 26.6) and more variable
(SD = 9.2) than the normative group (M = 18.1, SD = 5.9; see
Table 1). The corresponding differences for PA (patient group
Л/ = 32.5, SD = 7.5; normative group Μ = 35.0, SD = 6.4)
were also statistically significant because of the very large n of
the normative sample, but it would be premature to accept a
mean scale difference of 2.5 points as clinically meaningful
without further study.

Factorial Validity

Scale validity. An important step in evaluating the PANAS
scales is to demonstrate that they adequately capture the under­
lying mood factors. To do this, we subjected ratings on the 60
Zevon and Tellegen (1982) mood descriptors in each of the six
large data sets to a principal factor analysis with squared multi­
ple correlations as the communality estimates. Two dominant
factors emerged in each solution. Together, they accounted for
roughly two thirds of the common variance, ranging from
62.8% in the moment solution to 68.7% in the general ratings.
The first two factors in each solution were then rotated to or­
thogonal simple structure according to the varimax criterion.

Each of the six solutions generated two sets of factor scoring
weights that can be used to compute regression estimates of the

underlying PA and NA factors in those data. Within each data
set, we then correlated these estimated factor scores with the
PANAS PA and NA scales. The results, shown in Table 4, dem­
onstrate the expected convergent/discriminant pattern: Both
PANAS scales are very highly correlated with their correspond­
ing regression-based factor scores in each solution, with conver­
gent correlations ranging from .89 to .95, whereas the discrimi­
nant correlations are quite low, ranging from -.02 to -. 18.

Item validity. It is also important to demonstrate the factorial
validity of the individual PANAS items. To do this, we factored
subjects’ ratings on the 20 PANAS descriptors in each of the
six data sets; as before, we used a principal factor analysis with
squared multiple correlations as the initial communality esti­
mates. Because the PANAS terms were selected to be relatively
pure factor markers, it is not surprising that two dimensions
accounted for virtually all of the common variance in these so­
lutions (ranging from 87.4% in the moment data to 96.1 % in
the general ratings).

Median varimax loadings for the PANAS terms on these two
factors are presented in Table 5. All of the descriptors have
strong primary loadings (.50 and above) on the appropriate fac­
tor, and the secondary loadings are all acceptably low. Thus, all
of the PANAS items are good markers of their corresponding
factors.

Rating scale effects. The data shown in Tables 1 through 5
are all based on the same 5-point rating scale. Because the sub­
jects were instructed to rate the extent to which they experi­
enced each mood state, this may be termed an extent format. It
seems reasonable to ask, however, whether different response
formats might yield different results. Warr et al. (1983) have
presented data indicating that the correlation between PA and
NA scales varies according to the response scale used. Specifi­
cally, their PA and NA scales were highly correlated when they
used a frequency-type format in which subjects rated the pro­
portion of time they had experienced each mood state during a
specified time period.

To test the effect of rating format, we collected ratings on 54
mood terms in two different student samples, both using past
few weeks time instructions. In the first sample, 413 subjects
rated their mood using the usual extent rating format. In the
second, 338 students rated themselves on a 4-point frequency

Table 4
Correlations Between the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule
(PANAS) Scales and Scores of the First Two
Varimax Factors in Each Sample

Time
instructions n

PANAS PA scale
correlations

PANAS NA scale
correlations

Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 1 Factor 2

Moment 660 -.02 .95 .91 -.15
Today 657 -.02 .95 .93 -.11
Past few days 1,002 -.15 .92 .93 -.10
Past few weeks 586 -.10 .92 .92 -.18
Year 649 -.17 .89 .93 -.09
General 663 -.08 .94 .93 -.12

Note. Factor analyses are based on the set of 60 mood terms reported
in Zevon & Tellegen ( 1982). PA = Positive Affect. NA = Negative Affect.

DEVELOPMENT AND VALIDATION OF THE PANAS SCALES 1067

Th
is

do
cu

m
en

t i
s c

op
yr

ig
ht

ed
b

y
th

e
A

m
er

ic
an

P
sy

ch
ol

og
ic

al
A

ss
oc

ia
tio

n
or

o
ne

o
f i

ts
al

lie
d

pu
bl

ish
er

s.
Th

is
ar

tic
le

is
in

te
nd

ed
so

le
ly

fo
r t

he
p

er
so

na
l u

se
o

f t
he

in
di

vi
du

al
u

se
r a

nd
is

n
ot

to
b

e
di

ss
em

in
at

ed
b

ro
ad

ly
.

Loading on

Table 5
Median Varimax-Rotated Factor Loadings of the Positive
and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS)
Descriptors Across the Six Solutions

PANAS
descriptor

Positive
Affect

Negative
Affect

Enthusiastic .75 -.12
Interested .73 -.07
Determined .70 -.01
Excited .68 .00
Inspired .67 -.02
Alert .63 -.10
Active .61 -.07
Strong .60 -.15
Proud .57 -.10
Attentive .52 -.05
Scared .01 .74
Afraid .01 .70
Upset -.12 .67
Distressed -.16 .67
Jittery .00 .60
Nervous -.04 .60
Ashamed -.12 .59
Guilty -.06 .55
Irritable -.14 .55
Hostile -.07 .52

format (the options were little or none of the time, some of the
time, a good part of the time, and most ofthe time).

In addition to the PANAS terms, the mood descriptors used
in these samples allowed us to compare the factorial validity of
our scales with those of other investigators. In both samples, we
were able to measure the brief NA and PA scales developed by
Diener and Emmons (1984, Studies 3 through 5; see also Diener
& Iran-Nejad, 1986; Diener & Larsen, 1984; Diener et al.,
1985), Stone and his associates (Hedges et al., 1985; Stone,
1987; Stone et al., 1985), and McAdams and Constantian
(1983). Further, in the extent sample, 301 subjects rated them­
selves on Bradbum’s (1969) widely used NA and PA scales;
these were replaced by Warr et al.’s (1983) revised measures in
the frequency sample.

The ratings in each sample were subjected to separate princi­
pal factor analyses with squared multiple correlations in the di­
agonal (these analyses are reported in detail in Watson, in
press). Two large factors emerged in each solution, accounting
for 75.4% and 73.3% of the common variance in the extent and
frequency data, respectively. The first two factors in each solu­
tion were therefore rotated using varimax.

Table 6 presents correlations between the various mood
scales and regression estimates of these factors. Considering first
the PANAS scales, Table 6 demonstrates that they have excel­
lent factorial validity even when a frequency response format is
used: In both samples the convergent correlations are above .90
and the discriminant coefficients are all low. Thus, while we
prefer an extent-type rating scale, other response formats can
be used without diminishing the factorial validity of the scales.

Table 6 also demonstrates that the PANAS scales compare
favorably with other brief affect measures. With the exception
of the Bradburn scales, all of the mood scales have good conver­

gent correlations (i.e., .76 to .92) with the appropriate factor,
but none are higher than the corresponding values for the PA­
NAS scales. Thus, in terms of convergent validity, most of these
scales are reasonable approximations of the underlying factors,
although some are clearly more precise representations than
others. The discriminant correlations vary widely, however, es­
pecially in the frequency-format data, where many of the co­
efficients exceed —.30; across both samples, only the PANAS
scales have discriminant correlations consistently under -.20.
Overall, the PANAS scales offer the clearest convergent/dis-
criminant pattern of any pair.

In summary, the PANAS scales provide reliable, precise, and
largely independent measures of Positive Affect and Negative
Affect, regardless of the subject population studied or the time
frame and response format used.

External Validity

Correlations with measures of distress and psychopathology.
It is also interesting to examine correlations between the PA­
NAS scales and measures of related constructs, such as state
anxiety, depression, and general psychological distress (for an
extended discussion of how Positive and Negative Affect relate
to anxiety, depression, and general psychological dysfunction,
see Tellegen, 1985; Watson & Clark, 1984). We have used the
PANAS scales in conjunction with a number of other com­
monly used measures and report here on three of them: the
Hopkins Symptom Checklist (HSCL; Derogatis, Lipman, Rick-

Table 6
Correlations Between Various Positive Affect (PA) and Negative
Affect (NA) Mood Scales and the Factor Scores From the
Extent- and Frequency-Format Data

Extent format
Frequency

format

Factor Factor Factor Factor
Mood scale 1 2 1 2

Positive Affect scales
PANAS .92 -.08 .92 -.12
Diener & Emmons ( 1984) .89 -.22 .87 -.36
McAdams & Constantian

(1983) .90 -.19 .86 -.31
Stone, Hedges, Neale, &

Satin (1985) .88 -.04 .81 -.20
Warr, Barter, &

Brownbridge (1983) — — .81 -.30
Bradbum (1969) .50 -.18 — —

Negative Affect scales
PANAS -.08 .94 -.16 .91
Diener & Emmons (1984) -.21 .92 -.35 .89
McAdams & Constantian

(1983) -.20 .81 -.43 .76
Stone, Hedges, Neale, &

Satin (1985) .06 .84 -.11 .81
Warr, Barter, &

Brownbridge (1983) — — -.32 .79
Bradburn (1969) -.21 .51 — —

Note. ns with the extent-format factors ranged from 301 to 413. ns with
the frequency-format factors ranged from 336 to 338. PANAS = Posi­
tive and Negative Affect Schedule.

1068 D. WATSON, L. CLARK, AND A. TELLEGEN

Th
is

do
cu

m
en

t i
s c

op
yr

ig
ht

ed
b

y
th

e
A

m
er

ic
an

P
sy

ch
ol

og
ic

al
A

ss
oc

ia
tio

n
or

o
ne

o
f i

ts
al

lie
d

pu
bl

ish
er

s.
Th

is
ar

tic
le

is
in

te
nd

ed
so

le
ly

fo
r t

he
p

er
so

na
l u

se
o

f t
he

in
di

vi
du

al
u

se
r a

nd
is

n
ot

to
b

e
di

ss
em

in
at

ed
b

ro
ad

ly
.

Table 7
Correlations Between the Positive and Negative Affect
Schedule (PANAS) Scales and the Hopkins Symptom
Checklist (HSCL), Beck Depression Inventory (BDI),
and STAI State Anxiety Scale (A-State)

Measure and PANAS
time instructions n

Correlations with

PANAS NA PANAS PA

HSCL
Past few weeks 398 .74 -.19
Today® 53 .65 -.29

BDI
Past few days 880 .56 -.35
Past few weeks 208 .58 -.36

A-State
Past few weeks 203 .51 -.35

Note. Unless otherwise noted, subjects are college students. PA = Posi­
tive Affect. NA = Negative Affect.
• Normal adult sample.

els, Uhlenhuth, & Covi, 1974), the Beck Depression Inventory
(BDI; Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961), and
the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory State Anxiety Scale (A-State;
Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970).

The HSCL (Derogatis et al., 1974) is a measure of general
distress and dysfunction. Subjects rate the extent to which they
have experienced each of 58 symptoms or problems during the
past week. The HSCL and a subsequent 90-item version, the
SCL-90 (Derogatis, Rickels, & Rock, 1976), have been used fre­
quently as measures of clinical symptomatology in both normal
and clinical populations (e.g., Gotlib, 1984; Kanner et al., 1981;
Rickels, Lipman, Garcia, & Fisher, 1972). Although the HSCL
and SCL-90 each contain several subscales, analyses have re­
peatedly shown that both instruments reflect a large general dis­
tress factor (e.g., Dinning & Evans, 1977; Gotlib, 1984).

The BDI (Beck et al., 1961 ) is a 2 1-item self-report measure
of depressive symptomatology. Subjects rate whether they have
experienced each symptom during the past few days. The BDI
is commonly used to assess mild to moderate levels of depres­
sion, and studies have generally supported its validity in this
context (e.g., Bumberry, Oliver, & McClure, 1978; Coyne &
Gotlib, 1983; Hammen, 1980).

The A-State (Spielberger et al., 1970) is a 20-item scale that
asks subjects to rate their current affect. Researchers have used
the A-State to study subjects’ responses to a variety of stressful
and aversive events, including surgery, shock, pain, failure, criti­
cism, interviews, and exams (see Watson & Clark, 1984).

Correlations between the PANAS scales and the HSCL, BDI,
and A-State are presented in Table 7. Looking first at the HSCL,
Table 7 indicates that it is largely a measure of NA, although it
also shows modest (negative) correlations with PA. In fact, the
correlations between the HSCL and the PANAS NA scale are
high enough to suggest that the two measures are roughly inter­
changeable, at least in normal populations. Insofar as this is the
case, the PANAS NA scale seems to offer a shorter (10 vs. 58
items), simpler, and conceptually more straightforward mea­
sure of general psychological distress.

The BDI is also substantially correlated with the PANAS NA
scale, but the coefficients are not so high as to indicate inter­

changeability. In addition, the BDI has significant (negative)
correlations with PA, consistent with previous findings that de­
pressive symptomatology is affectively complex (Tellegen, 1985;
Watson & Clark, 1984; Watson, Clark, & Carey, in press). That
is, it involves the lack of pleasurable experiences (low PA) in
addition to anger, guilt, apprehension, and general psychologi­
cal distress (high NA). The PANAS scales offer the advantage
of providing reliable and independent measures of these two
affective components. Researchers interested in studying de­
pressed affect might therefore want to use the PANAS scales as
a complement to more traditional depression measures.

The A-State is also a mixture of high NA and low PA, repli­
cating the results of Watson and Clark (1984, Table 4) using
NA and PA factor scores. An inspection of the A-State’s items
indicates why this is the case. Many of the items tap mood states
traditionally associated with anxiety (e.g., feeling tense, upset,
worried, anxious, nervous, jittery, and highstrung) or its absence
(e.g., feeling calm, relaxed, and content), and such items will
produce a substantial correlation with the PANAS NA scale.
Other (reverse-keyed) items, however, reflect pleasant or high
PA states (e.g., feeling joyful, pleasant, self-confident, and
rested) that account for the A-State’s significant correlation
with PA. The A-State has repeatedly demonstrated its useful­
ness as a sensitive measure of unpleasant mood states; but, as
with the BDI, the PANAS scales offer the advantage of assessing
these two affective components separately.

Intraindividual analyses of nontest correlates.1 When used
with short-term time frame instructions (i.e., moment or to­
day), the PANAS scales are sensitive to changing internal or ex­
ternal circumstances. We have used the PANAS scales in three
large scale within-subjects investigations that illustrate their
usefulness in studying qualitatively distinctive intraindividual
mood fluctuations. In the first (Watson, 1988), 80 subjects com­
pleted a PANAS questionnaire each evening for 5-7 weeks, us­
ing today time instructions. At each assessment the subjects also
estimated their social activity (number of hours spent with
friends that day) and rated the level of stress they had experi­
enced. A total of 3,554 measurements were collected (Μ = 44.4
per subject). As hypothesized, within-subject variations in per­
ceived stress were strongly correlated with fluctuations in NA
but not in PA. Also, as expected, social activity was more highly
related to PA than to NA.

1 The data reported in Watson ( 1988) and Clark and Watson ( 1986)
are based on PA and NA factor scores. We have reanalyzed these data
using the PANAS scales and have obtained virtually identical results.

The other two studies were primarily concerned with diurnal
variation in mood. Clark and Watson (1986) had 123 subjects
fill out a PANAS form every 3 waking hours for a week using
moment time instructions. Subjects also rated their current
stress and noted whether they had been interacting socially
within the past hour. A total of5,476 assessments were collected
(Μ = 44.9 per subject). Leeka (1987) replicated this design with
an additional 73 subjects (a total of 3,206 measurements; Μ =
43.9 per subject). In both studies, perceived stress was again
consistently correlated with intraindividual fluctuations in NA
but not in PA. And, as before, social interaction was more
strongly related to PA than to NA.

PA also showed a strong time-of-day effect in both studies.
Specifically, PA scores tended to rise throughout the morning,

DEVELOPMENT AND VALIDATION OF THE PANAS SCALES 1069

Th
is

do
cu

m
en

t i
s c

op
yr

ig
ht

ed
b

y
th

e
A

m
er

ic
an

P
sy

ch
ol

og
ic

al
A

ss
oc

ia
tio

n
or

o
ne

o
f i

ts
al

lie
d

pu
bl

ish
er

s.
Th

is
ar

tic
le

is
in

te
nd

ed
so

le
ly

fo
r t

he
p

er
so

na
l u

se
o

f t
he

in
di

vi
du

al
u

se
r a

nd
is

n
ot

to
b

e
di

ss
em

in
at

ed
b

ro
ad

ly
.

remain steady during the rest of the day, and then decline again
during the evening. However, NA did not exhibit a significant
diurnal pattern in either sample.

Conclusion

We have presented information regarding the development of
brief scales to measure the two primary dimensions of mood­
Positive and Negative Affect. Whereas existing scales are unreli­
able, have poor convergent or discriminant properties, or are
cumbersome in length, these 10-item scales are internally con­
sistent and have excellent convergent and discriminant corre­
lations with lengthier measures of the underlying mood factors.
They also demonstrate appropriate stability over a 2-month
time period. When used with short-term instructions (e.g., right
now or today), they are sensitive to fluctuations in mood,
whereas they exhibit traitlike stability when longer-term in­
structions are used (e.g., past year or general). The scales corre­
late at predicted levels with measures of related constructs and
show the same pattern of relations with external variables that
have been seen in other studies. For example, the PA scale (but
not the NA scale) is related to social activity and shows signifi­
cant diurnal variation, whereas the NA scale (but not the PA
scale) is significantly related to perceived stress and shows no
circadian pattern.

Thus, we offer the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule as
a reliable, valid, and efficient means for measuring these two
important dimensions of mood.

References
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, Μ., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J.

(1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 4, 561-571.

Beiser, Μ. (1974). Components and correlates of mental well-being.
Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 15, 320-327.

Bradburn, N. Μ. (1969). The structure of psychological well-being. Chi­
cago: Aldine.

Brenner, B. ( 1975). Enjoyment as a preventive of depressive affect. Jour­
nal of Community Psychology, 3, 346-357.

Bumberry, W, Oliver, J. Μ., & McClure, J. (1978). Validation of the
Beck Depression Inventory in a university population using psychiat­
ric estimate as a criterion. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol­
ogy, 46, 150-155.

Clark, L. A„ & Watson, D. (1986, August). Diurnal variation in mood:
Interaction with daily events and personality. Paper presented at the
meeting of the American Psychological Association, Washington,
DC.

Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1988). Mood and the mundane: Relations
between daily life events and self-reported mood. Journal of Personal­
ity and Social Psychology, 54, 296-308.

Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Influence of extraversion and
neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 668-678.

Coyne, J. C„ & Gotlib, I. H, ( 1983). The role of cognition in depression:
A critical appraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 472-505.

Derogatis, L. R., Lipman, R. S., Rickels, K., Uhlenhuth, E. H., & Covi,
L. (1974). The Hopkins Symptom Checklist (HSCL): A self-report
symptom inventory. Behavioral Science, 19, 1-15.

Derogatis, L. R., Rickels, K., & Rock, A. (1976), The SCL-90 and the
MMPI: A step in the validation of a new self-report scale. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 128, 280-289.

Dienet; E., & Emmons, R. A. ( 1984). The independence of positive and

negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47,
1105-1117.

Diener, E., & Iran-Nejad, A. ( 1986). The relationship in experience be­
tween various types of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psy­
chology, 50, 1031-1038.

Diener, E., & Larsen, R. J. (1984). Temporal stability and cross-situa­
tional consistency of affective, behavioral, and cognitive responses.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 871-883.

Diener, E., Larsen, R. J., Levine, S., & Emmons, R. A. (1985). Intensity
and frequency: Dimensions underlying positive and negative affect.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1253-1265.

Dinning, W. D., & Evans, R. G. (1977). Discriminant and convergent
validity of the SCL-90 in psychiatric inpatients. Journal of Personal­
ity Assessment. 41, 304-310.

Epstein, S. (1979). The stability of behavior: I. On predicting most of
the people much of the time. Journal of Personality and Social Psy­
chology, 37. 1097-1126.

Gotlib, I. H. ( 1984). Depression and general psychopathology in univer­
sity students. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 19-30.

Hall, C. A. (1977). Differential relationships of pleasure and distress
with depression and anxiety over a past, present, and future time

framework. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minne­
sota, Minneapolis.

Hammen, C. L. (1980). Depression in college students: Beyond the
Beck Depression Inventory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy­
chology, 48, 126-128.

Harding, S. D. (1982). Psychological well-being in Great Britain: An
evaluation of the Bradburn Affect Balance Scale. Personality and In­
dividual Differences, 3, 167-175.

Hedges, S. Μ., Jandorf, L., & Stone, A. A. (1985). Meaning of daily
mood assessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48,
428-434.

Kammann, R., Christie, D., Irwin, R-, & Dixon, G. (1979). Properties
of an inventory to measure happiness (and psychological health). New
Zealand Psychologist, 8, 1-9.

Kanner, A. D., Coyne, J. C., Schaefer, C., & Lazarus, R. S. (1981). Com­
parison of two modes of stress measurement: Daily hassles and uplifts
versus major life events. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 1-39.

Leeka, J. ( 1987). Patterns of diurnal variation in mood in depressed and
nondepressed college students. Unpublished master’s thesis, Southern
Methodist University, Dallas, TX.

McAdams, D. P., & Constantian, C. A. (1983). Intimacy and affiliation
motives in daily living: An experience sampling analysis. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 851-861.

Moriwaki, S. Y. (1974). The Affect Balance Scale: A validity study with
aged samples. Journal of Gerontology, 29. 73-78.

Rickels, K,, Lipman, R. S., Garcia, C. R., &Fisher, E. (1972). Evaluat­
ing clinical improvement in anxious outpatients. American Journal
of Psychiatry, 128, 119-123.

Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personal­
ity and Social Psychology, 39, 1161-1178.

Russell, J. A. (1983). Pancultural aspects of the human conceptual orga­
nization of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
45. 1281-1288.

Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L„ & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual
for the Slate-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psy­
chologists Press.

Stone, A. A. ( 1981 ). The association between perceptions of daily expe­
riences and self- and spouse-rated mood. Journal of Research in Per­
sonality. 15, 510-522.

Stone, A. A. (1987). Event content in a daily survey is differentially
associated with concurrent mood. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 52, 56-58.

Stone, A. A., Hedges, S. Μ., Neale, J. Μ., & Satin, M. S. (1985). Pro­
spective and cross-sectional mood reports offer no evidence of a

1070 D. WATSON, L. CLARK, AND A. TELLEGEN

Th
is

do
cu

m
en

t i
s c

op
yr

ig
ht

ed
b

y
th

e
A

m
er

ic
an

P
sy

ch
ol

og
ic

al
A

ss
oc

ia
tio

n
or

o
ne

o
f i

ts
al

lie
d

pu
bl

ish
er

s.
Th

is
ar

tic
le

is
in

te
nd

ed
so

le
ly

fo
r t

he
p

er
so

na
l u

se
o

f t
he

in
di

vi
du

al
u

se
r a

nd
is

n
ot

to
b

e
di

ss
em

in
at

ed
b

ro
ad

ly
.

“Blue Monday” phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psy­
chology, 49, 129-134.

Tellegen, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personality and their rele­
vance to assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self-report. In A. H.
Tuma & J. D. Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and the anxiety disorders (pp.
681-706). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Tessier, R., & Mechanic, D. ( 1978). Psychological distress and perceived
health status. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 19. 254-262.

Warr, P. ( 1978). A study of psychological well-being. British Journal of
Psychology; 69, 111-121.

Warr, P., Barter, J., & Brownbridge, G. ( 1983). On the independence of
positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psy­
chology, 44, 644-651.

Watson, D. (in press). The vicissitudes of mood measurement: Effects
of varying descriptors, time frames, and response formats on mea­
sures of Positive and Negative Affect. Journal of Personality and So­
cial Psychology.

Watson, D. ( 1988). Intraindividual and interindividual analyses of Posi­
tive and Negative Affect: Their relation to health complaints, per­
ceived stress, and daily activities. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 54, 1020-1030.

Watson, D., &Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative Affectivity: The disposition
to experience aversive emotional states. Psychological Bulletin, 96,
465-490.

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Carey, G. (in press). Positive and Negative
Affectivity and their relation to anxiety and depressive disorders.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology.

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1984). Cross-cultural conver­
gence in the structure of mood: A Japanese replication and a compar­
ison with U.S. findings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
47. 127-144.

Watson, D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (in press). Health complaints, stress,
and distress: Exploring the central role of negative affectivity. Psycho­
logical Review.

Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985) Toward a consensual structure of
mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98. 219-235.

Wills, T. A. ( 1986). Stress and coping in early adolescence: Relationships
to substance use in urban school samples. Health Psychology, 5, 503­
529.

Zevon, Μ. A., & Tellegen, A. (1982). The structure of mood change: An
idiographic/nomothetic analysis. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 4 J, 111-122.

Appendix

The PANAS

This scale consists of a number of words that describe different feelings and emotions. Read each item and then mark
the appropriate answer in the space next to that word. Indicate to what extent [INSERT APPROPRIATE TIME
INSTRUCTIONS HERE]. Use the following scale to record your answers.

We have used PANAS with the following time instructions:

1
very slightly
or not at all

2
a little

3
moderately

4
quite a bit

5
extremely

_____ interested _____ irritable
_____ distressed _____ alert

excited ashamed
upset inspired
strong nervous

_____ guilty _____ determined
scared attentive

_____ hostile _____ jittery
enthusiastic active
proud afraid

Moment
Today
Past few days
Week
Past few weeks
Year
General

(you feel this way right now, that is, at the present moment)
(you have felt this way today)
(you have felt this way during the past few days)
(you have felt this way during the past week)
(you have felt this way during the past few weeks)
(you have felt this way during the past year)
(you generally feel this way, that is, how you feel on the average)

Received May 10, 1987
Revision received September 14,1987

Accepted November 11, 1987 ■

Writerbay.net

Looking for top-notch essay writing services? We've got you covered! Connect with our writing experts today. Placing your order is easy, taking less than 5 minutes. Click below to get started.


Order a Similar Paper Order a Different Paper